Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Table 4.1: Native VOT norms (in ms) for plosives in Korean and English. Korean figures are averaged over the nine native speaker participants, English figures are from Lisker and Abramson (1964:394).  and Horrocks 2002; Cho, Jun, and Ladefoged 2002; Kim 2004; Silva 2006a; Kang and Guion 2008), while fo onset has been shown to increase going from lenis to fortis to aspirated stops (Han and Weitzman 1970; Hardcastle 1973; Kagaya 1974; Han 1996; Ahn 1999; Lee and Jung 2000; Kim et al. 2002; Cho et al. 2002; Kim 2004; Silva 2006a; Kang and Guion 2008). The phonetic implementation of the Korean contrast, however, has been undergoing a change in the language, with younger speakers in- creasingly relying on fp to distinguish categories that used to be more distinguishable on the basis of VOT alone (Silva 2006a,b; Kang and Guion 2008). In each of these dimensions there is now considerable overlap between categories, such that VOT and fo are both necessary cues for making a full three-way contrast, schematized in Figure 4.1 on the basis of the perception and production data of Kim (2004). The stop pro- duction of the native Korean participants in the current study is consistent with this general phonetic space, as shown in Figure 4.2. Thus, in the Korean speech to which learner participants had the most exposure, the Korean laryngeal categories can be assumed to be realized as follows: fortis stops, with short-lag VOT and relatively high fp onset; lenis stops, with medium- to long-lag VOT and relatively low fo onset; and aspirated stops, with the longest VOT and the highest fp onset.  Tn contrast to the necessarv use of VOT and fain making a full three-wav contrast

Table 4 1: Native VOT norms (in ms) for plosives in Korean and English. Korean figures are averaged over the nine native speaker participants, English figures are from Lisker and Abramson (1964:394). and Horrocks 2002; Cho, Jun, and Ladefoged 2002; Kim 2004; Silva 2006a; Kang and Guion 2008), while fo onset has been shown to increase going from lenis to fortis to aspirated stops (Han and Weitzman 1970; Hardcastle 1973; Kagaya 1974; Han 1996; Ahn 1999; Lee and Jung 2000; Kim et al. 2002; Cho et al. 2002; Kim 2004; Silva 2006a; Kang and Guion 2008). The phonetic implementation of the Korean contrast, however, has been undergoing a change in the language, with younger speakers in- creasingly relying on fp to distinguish categories that used to be more distinguishable on the basis of VOT alone (Silva 2006a,b; Kang and Guion 2008). In each of these dimensions there is now considerable overlap between categories, such that VOT and fo are both necessary cues for making a full three-way contrast, schematized in Figure 4.1 on the basis of the perception and production data of Kim (2004). The stop pro- duction of the native Korean participants in the current study is consistent with this general phonetic space, as shown in Figure 4.2. Thus, in the Korean speech to which learner participants had the most exposure, the Korean laryngeal categories can be assumed to be realized as follows: fortis stops, with short-lag VOT and relatively high fp onset; lenis stops, with medium- to long-lag VOT and relatively low fo onset; and aspirated stops, with the longest VOT and the highest fp onset. Tn contrast to the necessarv use of VOT and fain making a full three-wav contrast