The empirical case for role-governed categories
2011, Cognition
https://doi.org/10.1016/J.COGNITION.2010.10.009Abstract
Most theories of categorization posit feature-based representations. Markman and Stilwell (2001) argued that many natural categories name roles in relational systems and therefore they are role-governed categories. There is little extant empirical evidence to support the existence of role-governed categories. Three experiments examine predictions for ways that role-governed categories should differ from feature-based categories. Experiment 1 shows that our knowledge of role-governed categories, in contrast to feature-based categories, is largely about properties extrinsic to category members. Experiment 2 shows that role-governed categories have more prominent ideals than feature-based categories. Experiment 3 demonstrates that novel role-governed categories are licensed by the instantiation of novel relational structures. We then discuss broader implications for the study of categories and concepts.
References (61)
- Ahn, W., Kim, N. S., Lassaline, M. E., & Dennis, M. (2000). Causal status as a determinant of feature centrality. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 361-416.
- Barr, R. A., & Caplan, L. J. (1987). Category representations and their implications for category structure. Memory and Cognition, 15, 397-418.
- Barsalou, L. W. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory and Cognition, 11, 211-227.
- Barsalou, L. W. (1985). Ideas, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structures in categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 629-654.
- Bowdle, B., & Gentner, D. (2005). The career of metaphor. Psychological Review, 112, 193-216.
- Brennan, S. E., & Clark, H. H. (1996). Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1482-1493.
- Chi, M., Feltovich, P., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cognitive Science, 5(2), 121-152.
- Clark, E. V., & Clark, H. H. (1979). When nouns surface as verbs. Language, 55(4), 767-811.
- Clark, H. H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. (1986). Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition, 22, 1-39.
- Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic memory. Psychological Review, 82, 407-428.
- Cree, G. S., McNorgan, C., & McRae, K. (2006). Distinctive features hold a privileged status in the computation of word meaning: Implications for theories of semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 32, 643-658.
- Cree, G. S., McRae, K., & McNorgan, C. (1999). An attractor model of lexical conceptual processing: Simulating semantic priming. Cognitive Science, 23, 371-414.
- Ferretti, T. R., McRae, K., & Hatherell, A. (2001). Integrating verbs, situation schemas, and thematic role concepts. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 516-547.
- Gentner, D. (1975). Evidence for the psychological reality of semantic components: The verbs of possession. In D. A. Norman, D. E. Rumelhart, & the LNR Research Group (Eds.), Explorations in cognition (pp. 211-246). San Francisco: Freeman.
- Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S. A. Kuczaj (Ed.). Language development: Language, thought and culture (Vol. 2, pp. 301-334). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
- Gentner, D. (1983). Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7, 155-170.
- Gentner, D. (1989). The mechanisms of analogical learning. In S. Vosniadou & A. Ortony (Eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning. New York: Cambridge University Press.
- Gentner, D., & Asmuth, J. (2008). Can relationality be distinguished from abstractness in noun mutability? In B. C. Love, K. McRae, & V. M. Sloutsky (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 863-868). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
- Gentner, D., Anggoro, F. K., & Klibanoff, R. S. (in press). Structure-mapping and relational language support children's learning of relational categories. Child Development.
- Gentner, D., & Kurtz, K. (2005). Relational categories. In W. K. Ahn, R. L. Goldstone, B. C. Love, A. B. Markman, & P. W. Wolff (Eds.), Categorization inside and outside the lab (pp. 151-175). Washington, DC: APA.
- Glushko, R. J., Maglio, P. P., Matlock, T., & Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Categorization in the wild. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 129-135.
- Goldwater, M. B., & Markman, A. B. (in press). Categorizing entities by common role. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
- Goldwater, M. B., Asmuth, J., & Gentner, D. (in preparation). Norms distinguishing schema-governed categories, role-governed categories, and feature-based categories.
- Goldwater, M. B., Markman, A. B., Trujilli, L., & Schnyer, D. (in preparation). An ERP analysis of licensing novel role-governed categories.
- Goldwater, M. B., & Markman, A. B. (2009). Constructional sources of implicit agents in sentence comprehension. Cognitive Linguistics, 20, 675-702.
- Holyoak, K. J., & Thagard, P. (1989). Analogical mapping by constraint satisfaction. Cognitive Science, 13, 295-355.
- Jackendoff, R. (1990). Semantic structures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Jackendoff, R. (2002). Foundations of language: Brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
- Jones, M., & Love, B. C. (2007). Beyond common features: The role of roles in determining similarity. Cognitive Psychology, 55, 196-231.
- Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Woolley, J. D. (1982). Paradigms and processes in reading comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 11, 228-238.
- Kaschak, M. P., & Glenberg, A. M. (2000). Constructing meaning: The role of affordances and grammatical constructions in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 508-529.
- Kay, P. (2005). Argument structure constructions and the argument- adjunct distinction. In M. Fried & H. C. Boas (Eds.), Grammatical constructions -Back to the roots. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins.
- Kay, P., & Fillmore, C. J. (1999). Grammatical constructions and linguistic generalizations: The what's X doing Y? construction. Language, 75, 1-33.
- Keil, F. C. (1989). Spiders in the web of belief: The tangled relations between concepts and theories. Mind and Language, 4, 43-45.
- Kim, A., & Osterhout, L. (2005). The independence of combinatory semantic processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 205-225.
- Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203-205.
- Levin, B., & Rappaport-Hovav, M. (2005). Argument realization. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
- Lin, E. L., & Murphy, G. L. (2001). Thematic relations in adults' concepts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 3-28.
- Love, B. C., Medin, D. L., & Gureckis, T. M. (2004). SUSTAIN: A network model of category learning. Psychological Review, 111, 309-332.
- Lynch, E. B., Coley, J. D., & Medin, D. L. (2000). Tall is typical: Central tendency, ideal dimensions and graded category structure among tree experts and novices. Memory and Cognition, 28, 41-50.
- Markman, A. B. (1999). Knowledge representation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
- Markman, A. B., & Makin, V. S. (1998). Referential communication and category acquisition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 331-354.
- Markman, A. B., & Ross, B. (2003). Category use and category learning. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 592-613.
- Markman, A. B., & Stilwell, C. H. (2001). Role-governed categories. Journal of Experimental & Theoretical Artificial Intelligence, 13, 329-358.
- Mauner, G., & Koenig, J. P. (2000). Linguistic vs. conceptual sources of implicit agents in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory & Language, 43, 110-234.
- McKoon, G., & MacFarland, T. (2002). Event templates in the lexical representations of verbs. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 1-44.
- McRae, K., Ferretti, T. R., & Amyote, L. (1997). Thematic roles as verb- specific concepts. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 137-176.
- Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). A context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85, 207-238.
- Murphy, G. L. (2002). The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Murphy, G. L., & Medin, D. L. (1985). The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review, 92, 289-316.
- Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identification- categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115(1), 39-57.
- Posner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 353-363.
- Rehder, B. (2003). Categorization as causal reasoning. Cognitive Science, 27, 709-748.
- Rehder, B., & Ross, B. H. (2001). Abstract coherent concepts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 1261-1275.
- Rein, J. R., Goldwater, M. B., & Markman, A. B. (2010). What is typical about the typicality effect in category-based induction? Memory and Cognition, 38, 377-388.
- Rosch, E. H. (1973). Natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 328-350.
- Ross, B. H., & Murphy, G. L. (1999). Food for thought: Cross-classification and category organization in a complex real-world domain. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 495-553.
- Sloman, S. A., Love, B. C., & Ahn, W. K. (1998). Feature centrality and conceptual coherence. Cognitive Science, 22, 189-228.
- Smith, E. E., Shoben, E. J., & Rips, L. J. (1974). Structure and process in semantic memory: A featural model for semantic decisions. Psychological Review, 81, 214-241.
- Traxler, M. J., Pickering, M. J., & McElree, B. (2002). Coercion in sentence processing: Evidence from eye-movements and self-paced reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 530-547.
- Wechsler, S. (1995). The semantic basis of argument structure: A study of the relationship between word meaning and syntax. Stanford University: CSLI Publications.