Relational Versus Absolute Representation in Categorization
2012, The American Journal of Psychology
https://doi.org/10.5406/AMERJPSYC.125.4.0481Abstract
This study explores relational-like and absolute-like representations in categorization. Although there is much evidence that categorization processes can involve information about both the particular physical properties of studied instances and abstract (relational) properties, there has been little work on the factors that lead to one kind of representation as opposed to the other. We tested 370 participants in 6 experiments, in which participants had to classify new items into predefined artificial categories. In 4 experiments, we observed a predominantly relational-like mode of classification, and in 2 experiments we observed a shift toward an absolute-like mode of classification. These results suggest 3 factors that promote a relational-like mode of classification: fewer items per group, more training groups, and the presence of a time delay. Overall, we propose that less information about the distributional properties of a category or weaker memory traces for the category exemplars (induced, e.g., by having smaller categories or a time delay) can encourage relational-like categorization.
References (45)
- Alluisi, E. A., & Sidorsky, R. C. (1958). The empirical validity of equal discriminability scaling. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 86-95.
- Anderson, J. R., & Betz, J. (2001). A hybrid model of catego- rization. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 629-647.
- Anderson, J. R., & Lebiere, C. (1998). The atomic components of thought. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Ashby, F. G., & Maddox, W. T. (1993). Relations between prototype, exemplar, and decision bound models of categorization. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 37, 372-400.
- Braida, L. D., & Durlach, N. I. (1972). Intensity perception. II. Resolution in one-interval paradigms. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 51, 483-502.
- Brown, S. D., Marley, A. A. J., Donkin, C., & Heathcote, A. J. (2008). An integrated, principled account of absolute identification. Psychological Review, 115, 396-425.
- Chater, N. (1999). The search for simplicity: A fundamental cognitive principle? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 273-302.
- Eriksen, C. W., & Hake, H. W. (1955). Absolute judgments as a function of stimulus range and number of stimulus and response categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 323-332.
- Gentner, D. (1983). Structure-mapping: A theoretical frame- work for analogy. Cognitive Science, 7, 155-170.
- Gentner, D. (1989). The mechanisms of analogical learning. In S. Vosniadou & A. Ortony (Eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning (pp. 199-241). London, England: Cambridge University Press.
- Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 306-355.
- Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 1-38.
- Goldstone, R. L. (1994). The role of similarity in categoriza- tion: Providing a groundwork. Cognition, 52, 125-157.
- Goldstone, R. L., & Barsalou, L. W. (1998). Reuniting per- ception and conception. Cognition, 65, 231-262.
- Holyoak, K. J., & Thagard, P. (1995). Mental leaps: Analogy in creative thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Homa, D., & Vosburgh, R. (1976). Category breadth and the abstraction of prototypical information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 322-330.
- Johansen, M. K., & Palmeri, T. J. (2002). Representational shifts in category learning. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 482-553.
- Jones, M., Love, B. C., & Maddox, W. T. (2006). Recency ef- fects as a window to generalization: Separating decisional and perceptual sequential effects in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 316-332.
- Lacouture, Y., Li, S., & Marley, A. A. J. (1998). The roles of stimulus and response set size in the identification and categorization of unidimensional stimuli. Australian Journal of Psychology, 50, 165-174.
- Laming, D. R. J. (1984). The relativity of "absolute" judge- ments. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 37, 152-183.
- Laming, D. R. J. (1997). The measurement of sensation. Lon- don, England: Oxford University Press.
- Lee, M. D., & Vanpaemel, W. (2008). Exemplars, prototypes, similarities and rules in category representation: An ex- ample of hierarchical Bayesian analysis. Cognitive Science, 32, 1403-1424.
- Lovett, M. C., & Anderson, J. R. (2005). Thinking as a pro- duction system. In K. J. Holyoak & R. Morrison (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 401-429). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
- Medin, D. L., Goldstone, R. L., & Gentner, D. (1993). Re- spects for similarity. Psychological Review, 100, 254-278.
- Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85, 207-238.
- Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for information processing. Psychological Review, 63, 81-97.
- Nakagawa, S. (2004). A farewell to Bonferroni: The problems of low statistical power and publication bias. Behavioral Ecology, 15, 1044-1045.
- Nosofsky, R. M. (1984). Choice, similarity, and the context theory of classification. Journal of Experimental Psychol- ogy: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 104-114.
- Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identifi- cation categorization relationship. Journal of Experimen- tal Psychology: General, 115, 39-57.
- Perneger, T. V. (1998). What's wrong with Bonferroni adjust- ments? British Medical Journal, 316, 1236-1238.
- Pollack, I. (1952). The information of elementary auditory displays. I. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 24, 745-749.
- Posner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of ab- stract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77, 353-363.
- Pothos, E. M., & Bailey, T. M. (2009). Predicting category in- tuitiveness with the rational model, the simplicity model, and the generalized context model. Journal of Experi- mental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1062-1080.
- Pothos, E. M., & Chater, N. (2002). A simplicity principle in unsupervised human categorization. Cognitive Science, 26, 303-343.
- Pothos, E. M., & Close, J. (2008). One or two dimensions in spontaneous classification: A simplicity approach. Cogni- tion, 107, 581-602.
- Pothos, E. M., Perlman, A., Bailey, T. M., Kurtz, K., Edwards, D. J., Hines, P., & McDonnell, J. V. (2011). Measuring cat- egory intuitiveness in unconstrained categorization tasks. Cognition, 121, 83-100.
- Reed, S. K. (1972). Pattern recognition and categorization. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 382-407.
- Rouder, J. N., & Ratcliff, R. (2004). Comparing categoriza- tion models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Gen- eral, 133, 63-82.
- Shin, H. J., & Nosofsky, R. M. (1992). Similarity-scaling stud- ies of "dot-pattern" classification and recognition. Jour- nal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 278-304.
- Stewart, N., Brown, G. D. A., & Chater, N. (2005). Absolute identification by relative judgment. Psychological Review, 112(4), 881-911.
- Tenenbaum, J., & Griffiths, T. L. (2001). Generalization, similarity, and Bayesian inference. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 629-641.
- Ward, L. M. (1987). Remembrance of sounds past: Memory and psychophysical scaling. Journal of Experimental Psy- chology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 216-227.
- Ward, L. M., & Lockhead, G. R. (1971). Response system processes in absolute judgment. Perception & Psychophys- ics, 9, 73-78.
- Weber, D. L., Green, D. M., & Luce, R. D. (1977). Effect of practice and distribution of auditory signals on absolute identification. Perception & Psychophysics, 22, 223-231.
- Zotov, V., Jones, M. N., & Mewhort, D. J. K. (2010). Contrast and assimilation in classification and exemplar produc- tion. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73, 621-639.