Intuitive and Deliberate Judgments Are Based on Common Principles
https://doi.org/10.1037/A0020762Abstract
A popular distinction in cognitive and social psychology has been between intuitive and deliberate judgments. This juxtaposition has aligned in dual-process theories of reasoning associative, unconscious, effortless, heuristic, and suboptimal processes (assumed to foster intuitive judgments) versus rule-based, conscious, effortful, analytic, and rational processes (assumed to characterize deliberate judgments). In contrast, we provide convergent arguments and evidence for a unified theoretical approach to both intuitive and deliberative judgments. Both are rule-based, and in fact, the very same rules can underlie both intuitive and deliberate judgments. The important open question is that of rule selection, and we propose a 2-step process in which the task itself and the individual's memory constrain the set of applicable rules, whereas the individual's processing potential and the (perceived) ecological rationality of the rule for the task guide the final selection from that set. Deliberate judgments are not generally more accurate than intuitive judgments; in both cases, accuracy depends on the match between rule and environment: the rules' ecological rationality. Heuristics that are less effortful and in which parts of the information are ignored can be more accurate than cognitive strategies that have more information and computation. The proposed framework adumbrates a unified approach that specifies the critical dimensions on which judgmental situations may vary and the environmental conditions under which rules can be expected to be successful.
Key takeaways
AI
AI
- Both intuitive and deliberate judgments rely on the same rule-based processes.
- A two-step rule selection process is proposed, involving task constraints and ecological rationality.
- Judgment accuracy depends on the ecological rationality of the applicable rules, not their complexity.
- Heuristics can yield more accurate judgments than complex analytical strategies under specific conditions.
- The text advocates for a unified theory of judgment, challenging dual-process paradigms.
References (115)
- Anderson, J. R. (1983). The structure of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- Asch, S. (1946). Forming impressions of personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41, 258 -290.
- Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York, NY: Basic Books.
- Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automaticity in social psychology. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic prin- ciples (pp. 169 -183). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
- Beilock, S. L., Bertenthal, B. I., McCoy, A. M., & Carr, T. H. (2004). Haste does not always make waste: Expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 373-379.
- Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., & Starkes, J. L. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: Impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sen- sorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 6 -16.
- Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2005). The origin and evolution of cultures. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
- Brighton, H. (2006). Robust inference with simple cognitive models. In C. Lebiere & R. Wray (Eds.), AAAI spring symposium: Cognitive science principles meet AI-hard problems (pp. 17-22). Menlo Park, CA: Amer- ican Association for Artificial Intelligence.
- Bröder, A. (2003). Decision making with the "adaptive toolbox": Influence of environmental structure, intelligence, and working memory load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cogni- tion, 29, 611-625.
- Bröder, A., & Newell, B. R. (2008). Challenging some common beliefs: Empirical work within the adaptive toolbox metaphor. Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 205-214.
- Bruner, J. S. (1973). Beyond the information given: Studies on the psy- chology of knowing. Oxford, England: W. W. Norton.
- Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116 -131.
- Carnap, R. (1947). Meaning and necessity. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
- Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychol- ogy. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
- Chater, N., Oaksford, M., Nakisa, R., & Redington, M. (2003). Fast, frugal and rational: How rational norms explain behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 90, 63-86.
- Chiu, C.-y., & Hong, Y.-y. (2006). Social psychology of culture. New York NY: Psychology Press.
- Chomsky, N., (1986). Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin, and use. Westport, CT: Prager.
- Chun, W. Y., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2006). The role of task demands and processing resources in the use of base rate and individuating informa- tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 205-217.
- Cokely, E. T., Parpart, P., & Schooler, L. J. (2009). There is no "the" heuristic system: Paradoxical effects of fluency, aging, and cognitive reflection. Unpublished manuscript.
- Czerlinski, J., Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (1999). How good are simple heuristics? In G. Gigerenzer, P. M. Todd, & the ABC Research Group, Simple heuristics that make us smart (pp. 97-118). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
- Dawes, R. H. (1979). The robust beauty of improper linear models in decision making. American Psychologist, 34, 571-582.
- Dawes, R. H., & Corrigan, B. (1974). Linear models in decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 95-106.
- DeMiguel, V., Garlappi, L., & Uppal, R. (2009). Optimal versus naive diversification: How inefficient is the 1/N portfolio strategy? Review of Financial Studies, 22, 1915-1953.
- Dieckmann, A., & Rieskamp, J. (2007). The influence of information redundancy on probabilistic inferences. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1801- 1813.
- Egidi, M., & Marengo, L. (2004). Near-decomposability, organization, and evolution: Some notes on Herbert Simon's contribution. In M. Augier & J. J. March (Eds.), Models of a man: Essays in memory of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 335-350). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1975). Unit weighting schemes for decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 13, 171-192.
- Epstein, S., Lipson, A., Holstein, C., & Huh, E. (1992). Irrational reactions to negative outcomes: Evidence for two conceptual systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 889 -906.
- Erb, H.-P., Kruglanski, A. W., Chun, W. Y., Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., & Spiegel, S. (2003). Searching for commonalities in human judgment: The parametric unimodel and its dual mode alternatives. European Review of Social Psychology, 14, 1-47.
- Evans, J. St. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judg- ment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255-278.
- Evans, J. St. B. T., Barston, J. L., & Pollard, P. (1983). On the conflict between logic and belief in syllogistic reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 11, 295-306.
- Fechner, G. T. (1966). Elements of psychophysics. New York, NY: Holt, Reinehart, & Winston. (Original work published 1860)
- Fiedler, K. (2007). Informational ecology and the explanation of social cognition and behavior. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (2007). Social psychology: A handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 176 - 200). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
- Funder, D. C. (1987). Errors and mistakes: Evaluating the accuracy of social judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 75-90.
- Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and proposi- tional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692-731.
- Gigerenzer, G. (2005). I think; therefore, I err. Social Research, 72, 195-218.
- Gigerenzer, G. (2007). Gut feelings: The intelligence of the unconscious. New York, NY: Viking.
- Gigerenzer, G. (2009). Surrogates for theory. APS Observer, 22(2), 21-23.
- Gigerenzer, G., & Brighton, H. (2009). Homo heuristicus: Why biased minds make better inferences. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 107-143.
- Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103, 650 - 669. Gigerenzer, G., & Murray, D. J. (1987). Cognition as intuitive statistics. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Gigerenzer, G., & Regier, T. (1996). How do we tell an association from a rule? Comment on Sloman (1996). Psychological Bulletin, 119, 23-26.
- Gigerenzer, G., & Selten, R. (Eds.). (2001). Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Goldstein, D. G., & Gigerenzer, G. (2002). Models of ecological rational- ity: The recognition heuristic. Psychological Review, 109, 75-90.
- Hahn, U., & Chater, N. (1998). Similarity and rules: Distinct? Exhaustive? Empirically distinguishable? Cognition, 65, 197-230.
- Helmholtz, J. A. (2000). Treatise on physiological optics. New York, NY: Dover. (Original work published 1910)
- Hertwig, R., Davis, J. R., & Sulloway, F. J. (2002). Parental investment: How an equity motive can produce inequality. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 728 -745.
- Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability and salience. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (1996). Social psychology: A handbook of basic processes (pp. 133-168). New York, NY: Guilford.
- Hogarth, R. M., & Karelaia, N. (2007). Heuristic and linear models of judgment: Matching rules and environments. Psychological Review, 114, 733-758.
- Holyoak, K. J., Kohl, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1989). A theory of conditioned reasoning: Inductive learning within rule-based hierarchies. Psycholog- ical Review, 96, 315-340.
- Hutchinson, J. M. C., & Gigerenzer, G. (2005). Simple heuristics and rules of thumb: Where psychologists and behavioural biologists might meet. Behavioural Processes, 69, 97-124.
- Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobio- graphical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306 -340.
- James, W. (1907). Pragmatism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- James, W. (1909). The meaning of truth. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni- versity Press.
- Johnson, E. L., & Goldstein, D. G. (2003). Do defaults save lives? Science, 302, 1338 -1339.
- Johnson, J. G., & Raab, M. (2003). Take the first: Option generation and resulting choices. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro- cesses, 91, 215-229.
- Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58, 697-720.
- Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80, 237-251.
- Keeney, R. L., & Raiffa, H. (1993). Decisions with multiple objectives- preferences and value tradeoffs. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
- Keren, G., & Schul, Y. (2009). Two is not always better than one: A critical evaluation of two-system theories. Perspectives on Psychological Sci- ence, 4, 500 -533.
- Kersten, D., Mammassian, P., & Yuille, A. (2004). Object perception as Bayesian inference. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 271-304.
- Kleffner, D. A., & Ramachandran, V. S. (1992). On the perception of shape from shading. Perception & Psychophysics, 52, 18 -36.
- Kruglanski, A. W. (2004). The psychology of closed mindedness. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
- Kruglanski, A. W., Erb, H. P., Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., & Chun. W. Y. (2006). On parametric continuities in the world of binary either ors. Psychological Inquiry 17, 153-165.
- Kruglanski, A. W., & Orehek, E. (2007). Partitioning the domain of human inference: Dual mode and system models and their alternatives. Annual Review of Psychology, 8, 291-316.
- Kruglanski, A. W., & Pierro, A. (2008). Night and day, you are the one: On circadian mismatches and the transference effect in social perception. Psychological Science, 19, 296 -301.
- Kruglanski, A. W., Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., & DeGrada, E. (2006). Groups as epistemic providers: Need for closure and the unfolding of group centrism. Psychological Review, 113, 84 -100.
- Kruglanski, A. W., Raviv, A., Bar-Tal, D., Raviv, A., Sharvit, K., Ellis, S., . . . Mannetti, L. (2005). Says who? Epistemic authority effects in social judgment. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy- chology (Vol. 37, pp. 346 -392). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
- Kruglanski, A. W., & Thomson, E. P. (1999a). The illusory second mode or, the cue is the message. Psychological Inquiry, 10, 182-193.
- Kruglanski, A. W., & Thomson, E. P. (1999b). Persuasion by a single route: A view from the unimodel. Psychological Inquiry, 10, 83-109.
- Kruglanski, A. W., & Webster, D. M. (1996). Motivated closing of the mind: "Seizing" and "freezing." Psychological Review, 103, 263-283.
- Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psy- chological Review, 95, 492-527.
- Marcus, G., Brinkmann, U., Clahsen, H., Wiese, R., & Pinker, S. (1995). German inflection: The exception that proves the rule. Cognitive Psy- chology, 29, 189 -256.
- Marewski, J. N., & Schooler, L. J. (2010). How memory aids strategy selection. Manuscript submitted for publication.
- Mata, R., Schooler, L., & Rieskamp, J. (2007). The aging decision maker: Cognitive aging and the adaptive selection of decision strategies. Psy- chology and Aging, 22, 796 -810.
- McArthur, L. Z., & Baron, R. M. (1983). Toward an ecological theory of social perception. Psychological Review, 90, 215-238.
- McGuire, W. J. (1960). A syllogistic analysis of cognitive relationships. In C. I. Hovland & M. J. Rosenberg (Eds.), Attitude organization and change: An analysis of consistency among attitude components (pp. 65-111). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
- Medin, D. L., & Ross, B. H. (1989). The specific character of abstract thought: Categorization, problem solving, and induction. In R. J. Stern- berg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 5, pp. 189 -223). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Mitchell, C. J., DeHouwer, J., & Lovibond, P. F. (2009). The propositional nature of human associative learning. Behavioral & Brain Science, 32, 183-198.
- Neal, D. T., Wood, W., & Quinn, J. M. (2006). Habits: A repeat perfor- mance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 198 -202.
- Ortmann, A., Gigerenzer, G., Borges, B., & Goldstein, D. G. (2008). The recognition heuristic: A fast and frugal way to investment choice? In C. R. Plott & V. L. Smith (Eds.), Handbook of experimental economics results (Vol. 1, No. 28, pp. 993-1003). Amsterdam, Holland: North- Holland.
- Osman, B. (2004), Quality assessment, verification, and validation of modeling and simulation applications. Proceedings of the 2004 Winter Simulation Conference, 122-129.
- Pachur, T., & Hertwig. (2006). On the psychology of the recognition heuristic: Retrieval primacy as a key determinant of its use. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 983- 1002.
- Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
- Petty, R. E., & Caccioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123-205). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
- Petty, R. E., Wells, G. L., & Brock, T. C. (1976). Distraction can enhance or reduce yielding to propaganda: Thought disruption versus effort justification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 874 - 884.
- Pichert, D., & Katsikopoulos, K. V. (2008). Green defaults: Information presentation and pro-environmental behavior. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28, 63-73.
- Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., Erb, H. P., Spiegel, S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2005). Informational length and order of presentation as determinants of persuasion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 458 -469.
- Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2004). Relevance override: On the reduced impact of "cues" under high moti- vation conditions of persuasion studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 251-264.
- Pizlo, Z. (2001). Perception viewed as an inverse problem. Vision Re- search, 41, 3145-3161.
- Rescorla, R. A. (1985). Conditioned inhibition and facilitation. In R. R. Miller & N. E. Spear (Eds.), Information processing as animals: Con- ditioned inhibition (pp. 299 -326). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Erlbaum.
- Rescorla, R. A., & Holland, P. C. (1982). Behavioral studies of associative learning in animals. Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 265-308.
- Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian condi- tioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonrein- forcement. In A. H. Black & W. F. Procasy (Eds.), Classical condition- ing II: Current theory and research (pp. 64 -99). New York, NY: Appleton Century Croft.
- Reyna, V. (2004). How people make decisions that involve risk: A dual- processes approach. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 60 -66.
- Rickard, T. C., Sin-Heng, J., & Pashler, H. (2008). Spacing and the transition from calculation to retrieval. Psychological Bulletin and Re- view, 15, 656 -661.
- Rieskamp, J., & Otto, P. E. (2006). SSL: A theory of how people learn to select strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 207-236.
- Rock, I. (1983). The logic of perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Scheibehenne, B., & Bröder, A. (2007). Predicting Wimbledon 2005 tennis results by mere player name recognition. International Journal of Fore- casting, 3, 415-426.
- Schooler, L., & Hertwig, R. (2005). How forgetting aids heuristic infer- ence. Psychological Review, 112, 610 -628.
- Serwe, S., & Frings, C. (2006). Who will win Wimbledon? The recognition heuristic in predicting sports events. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 321-332.
- Shaffer, D. M., Krauchunas, S. M., Eddy, M., & McBeath, M. K. (2004). How dogs navigate to catch Frisbees. Psychological Science, 15, 437- 441.
- Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3-22.
- Smith, D. G., Standing, L., & deMan, A. (1992). Verbal memory elicited by ambient odor. Perceptual Motor Skills, 72, 339 -343.
- Smith, E. R., & Branscombe, N. R. (1988). Category accessibility as implicit memory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 490 -504.
- Steele, C. M., & Josephs, R. A. (1990). Alcohol myopia: Its prized and dangerous effects. American Psychologist, 45, 921-933.
- Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220 -247.
- Swann, W. B. (1984). Quest for accuracy in person perception: A matter of pragmatics. Psychological Review, 91, 457-477.
- Takezawa, M., Gummerum, M., & Keller, M. (2006). A stage for the rational tail of the emotional dog: Roles of moral reasoning in group decision making. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27, 117-139.
- Tetlock, P. E. (1985). Accountability: A social check on the fundamental attribution error. Social Psychology Quarterly, 48, 227-236.
- Todd, P. M., & Miller, G. F. (1999). From pride and prejudice to persua- sion: Realistic heuristics for mate search. In G. Gigerenzer, P. M. Todd, & the ABC Research Group (Eds.), Simple heuristics that make us smart (pp. 287-308). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
- Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York, NY: Appleton Century Crofts.
- Tomasello, M. (2000). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cam- bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
- Tversky, A. (1972). Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. Psycho- logical Review, 79, 281-299.
- Volz, K. G., Schooler, L. J., Schubotz, R. I., Raab, M., Gigerenzer, G., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2006). Why you think Milan is larger than Modena: Neural correlates of the recognition heuristic. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1924 -1936.
- Wason, P. C. (1968). Reasoning about a rule. Quarterly Journal of Exper- imental Psychology, 20, 273-281.
- Webster, D. M., Richter, L., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1996). On leaping to conclusions when feeling tired: Mental fatigue effects on impression for- mation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 181-195.
- Wyer, R. S. Jr. (1970). Quantitative prediction of opinion and belief change: A further test of the subjective probability model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 559 -570.