Discontinuous reduplication: A typological sketch
2019, Talk given at ALT 13, Pavia
Sign up for access to the world's latest research
Abstract
AI
AI
This paper presents a typological analysis of discontinuous reduplication across various languages, exploring its distinct functions and common patterns. It categorizes the grammatical roles of reduplicative structures, illustrating their occurrences in lexical categories such as verbs, nouns, adjectives, and adverbs. The study identifies several functional properties, including intensity, habituality, and universal quantification, and discusses interposing elements that can occur within these reduplicative forms.
Key takeaways
AI
AI
- Discontinuous reduplication (DR) integrates non-adjacent morphological material between reduplicants and bases.
- The study examines DR across 93 languages, identifying 20 with at least one DR case.
- A total of 166 occurrences were tagged for five parameters, revealing 56 construction types.
- DR serves 16 functional roles, including intensity, plurality, and habituality, but lacks correlation with interposing elements.
- Standard reduplication (RED) encompasses DR, but DR conveys a narrower semantic subset.
Related papers
2002
The myriad processes of reduplication have often been romanticized as providing a 'blueprint'for phonology—a window into larger mechanisms of morphological expression and phonological computation. Eric Raimy's book, a revised version of his 1999 dissertation at the University of Delaware, presents an entirely new theoretical framework for the analysis of the various patterns of reduplication attested in the world's languages.
Ohio State University Working Papers …, 1986
Indian Journal of Language and Linguistics
The paper discusses in detail echo formations and expressives found in the Odia language and the strategies for forming them. The complex semantic structure as well as the wide semantic and conceptual space they occupy, make them complex categories that have been ignored both by Western and Asian linguists because they are a prototypical grammatical feature not fitting the traditional grammar writing (Abbi, 2018). Odia has morphological fixed segmentism with the variants /ph/ and /m/. Morphological fixed segmentism is a kind of affixation, and so it resembles affixing morphology generally. [ɡaɽɪ > ɡaɽɪ phaɽɪ ‘car and other transport’ ; ʧɪra > ʧɪra mɪra ‘ torn and such’ ] This paper endeavours to find the reason behind this particular choice of phonemes by Odia speakers. Furthermore, the paper investigates if Odia base-reduplicant structures follow the Syllable Contact Law (Vennemann, 1988). According to the Syllable Contact Law, sonority should fall across syllable boundarie...
The paper discusses in detail echo formations and expressives found in South Asian languages. Their structural features and their associated semantics across a wide range of languages indicate that these two are significant South Asian areal phenomena (Emeneau 1956, 1969). The complex semantic structure of these constructions as well as a wide semantic and conceptual space they occupy – make them complex categories which had been ignored both by western and Asian linguists because they are a-prototypical grammatical feature not fitting the traditional grammar writing. Considering the effects of globalization, it is observed that while echo formations are stable structures which are indicators of social cohesion and bonding and thus find their inroads into ever-evolving languages, expressives are endangered structures and are dying very fast. Keywords: semantically elaborate grammatical category, endangered structures, replacer syllable, reduplicator
Journal of Universal Language
The main goal of the paper is showing that there are three different kinds of iterative phenomena in languages: phoneme reduplication, not analyzed here, reduplication, and repetition. The phenomena differ on the basis of the grammatical components involved and therefore have very different effects. My work argues that reduplication is first and foremost a formal phenomenon. It can involve several kinds of meaning, some of which of very iconic origin, but all the meanings get encoded grammatically. Then, phrases can be iterated, as well, and they are candidates for repetition. I take repetition to have an exclusively iconic function, basically with a single meaning: emphasis. No formal aspects are involved here. I insert the preceding generalization in the wider framework of the Parallel Architecture (Jackendoff 1997, 2002).
In Douthwaite J., PezzinI D. Words in Action: Diachronic and Synchronic Approaches to English Discourse: Studies in Honour of Ermanno Barisone.. Genova: ECTS ., 2008
This paper investigates the phenomenon of English reduplicativesof the type chit-chat, dilly-dally or hurly-burly and nitty-gritty, which appear significant on various aspects, e.g. 1) as instances of extragrammatical (or expressive) morphology, 2) as complex cases in terms of naturalness/iconicity, 3) as current processes for slangy formations, 4) as lexical devices for covering areas of morphopragmatic meanings and 5) as cases of likely difficulty in the process of translation. In this first report, only points (1) and (2) are touched upon. The topic deserves attention in linguistics, because English, like many other world languages, but unlike other Western European languages, 2 widely and productively exploits reduplication as a word formation mechanism. English reduplicatives are lexical items, contributing to the enrichment of the lexicon (not only in terms of connotations), whereas in other world languages, the mechanism more often has a functional motivation: it may express a variety of grammatical functions, from plurality to tense shifting, 3 diminutive, etc. Although difficult to describe in terms of rules, and for that reason highly neglected by grammarians, reduplicatives are by no means out of the ordinary: they are lively, productive and widespread, and they have been so for quite a number of centuries. The so-called Copy reduplicatives, i.e. based on identical member repetition (as in ha-ha), are recorded in some OE documents dating back to the year 1000, while the ablaut (riff-raff) and rhyming (hocus pocus) types appear to be fully established by the end of the sixteenth century (Minkova 2002: 133).
Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 2007
The focus of this rich volume is total reduplication, that is, reduplicative constructions where one finds two copies of the same element appearing adjacent to each other in a way prescribed by the grammar of a language, as opposed to representing mere repetition.1 To pick one example, in Swahili, total reduplication of an adjective can be used to code plurality of the noun it modifies (p. 3) rather than merely emphasizing its semantics. The book’s title both oversells and undersells its content. On the one hand, its areal focus is limited to Europe. While examples are drawn from languages beyond the continent, one will not find a global-scale survey of the sort that has become familiar since the publication of HASPELMATH ET AL. (2005). On the other hand, there is much more than areal linguistics here. Extensive attention is given to methodological issues, the literature review brings interesting older work to light, and, most importantly, the book convincingly demonstrates that the understudied phenomenon of total reduplication should be brought solidly into the fold of linguistic patterns worthy of descriptive and typological attention as part of grammatical “canon”. This last point is especially significant given that recent decades have seen much more attention paid to partial reduplication than total reduplication.

Loading Preview
Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.