Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Outline

Grouping does not require attention

2006, Perception & Psychophysics

References (49)

  1. Abrams, R. A., & Law, M. B. (2000). Object-based visual attention with endogenous orienting. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 818-833.
  2. Baylis, G. C., & Driver, J. (1992). Visual parsing and response com- petition: The effect of grouping factors. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 145-162.
  3. Baylis, G. C., & Driver, J. (1993). Visual attention and objects: Evi- dence for hierarchical coding of location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 19, 451-470.
  4. Behrmann, M., Zemel, R. S., & Mozer, M. C. (1998). Object-based attention and occlusion: Evidence from normal participants and a computational model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 24, 1011-1036.
  5. Ben-Av, M. B., Sagi, D., & Braun, J. (1992). Visual attention and per- ceptual grouping. Perception & Psychophysics, 52, 277-294.
  6. Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1998). Consciousness and neuroscience. Cere- bral Cortex, 8, 97-107.
  7. Dewar, R. E. (1967). Stimulus determinants of the magnitude of the Mueller-Lyer illusion. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 24, 708-710.
  8. Driver, J., & Baylis, G. C. (1989). Movement and visual attention: The spotlight metaphor breaks down. Journal of Experimental Psychol- ogy: Human Perception & Performance, 15, 448-456.
  9. Duncan, J. (1984). Selective attention and the organization of visual information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 501- 517.
  10. Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96, 433-458.
  11. Egeth, H. E., & Lamy, D. (2003). Attention. In I. B. Weiner (Series Ed.)
  12. & J. R. Graham & J. A. Naglieri (Vol. Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of psychology: Vol. 4. Experimental psychology (pp. 269-292). New York: Wiley.
  13. Egeth, H. E., & Yantis, S. (1997). Visual attention: Control, represen- tation and time course. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 269-297.
  14. Egly, R., Driver, J., & Rafal, R. D. (1994). Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: Evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 161-177.
  15. Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143-149.
  16. Greene, E., & Nelson, B. (1997). Evaluating Müller-Lyer effects using single fin-set configurations. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 293- 312.
  17. Handy, T. C., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). Attention and spatial selection: Electrophysiological evidence for modulation by perceptual load. Per- ception & Psychophysics, 62, 175-186.
  18. Handy, T. C., Soltani, M., & Mangun, G. R. (2001). Perceptual load and visuocortical processing: Event-related potentials reveal sensory- level selection. Psychological Science, 12, 213-218.
  19. Kramer, A. F., & Jacobson, A. (1991). Perceptual organization and focused attention: The role of objects and proximity in visual process- ing. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 267-284.
  20. Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness are differ- ent. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 12-18.
  21. Lamy, D. (2000). Object-based selection under focused attention: A fail- ure to replicate. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 1272-1279.
  22. Lamy, D., & Egeth, H. (2002). Object-based selection: The role of at- tentional shifts. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 52-66.
  23. Lamy, D., & Tsal, Y. (2000). Object features, object locations and object files: Which does selective attention activate and when? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 26, 1387-1400.
  24. Lamy, D., & Tsal, Y. (2001). On the status of location in visual selective attention. European Journal of Psychology, 13, 305-342.
  25. Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 21, 451-468.
  26. Lavie, N., & de Fockert, J. W. (2003). Contrasting effects of sensory limits and capacity limits in visual selective attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 202-212.
  27. Lavie, N., & Driver, J. (1996). On the spatial extent of attention in object- based visual selection. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 1238-1251.
  28. Lavie, N., & Tsal, Y. (1994). Perceptual load as a major determinant of the locus of selection in visual attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 183-197.
  29. Law, M. B., & Abrams, R. A. (2002). Object-based selection within and beyond the focus of spatial attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 1017-1027.
  30. Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  31. Mack, A., Tang, B., Tuma, R., Kahn, S., & Rock, I. (1992). Perceptual organization and attention. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 475-501.
  32. Moore, C. M., & Egeth, H. E. (1997). Perception without attention: Evidence of grouping under conditions of inattention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 23, 339-352.
  33. Moore, C. M., Yantis, S., & Vaughan, B. (1998). Object-based visual selection: Evidence from perceptual completion. Psychological Sci- ence, 9, 104-110.
  34. Mountjoy, P. T. (1958). Effects of exposure time and intertrial interval upon decrement to the Müller-Lyer illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56, 97-102.
  35. Mukerji, N. (1957). Is "illusion" illusion? Journal of General Psychol- ogy, 57, 209-212.
  36. Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353-383.
  37. Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century- Crofts.
  38. Norman, D. A., & Bobrow, D. G. (1975). On data-limited and resource- limited processes. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 44-64.
  39. Palmer, S., & Rock, I. (1994). Rethinking perceptual organization: The role of uniform connectedness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 29-55.
  40. Pollack, R. H. (1970). Magnitude of the Müller-Lyer illusion as a func- tion of hue in the absence of lightness contrast. Proceedings of the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, 5, 53-54.
  41. Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 446-461.
  42. Rock, I., Linnett, C. M., Grant, P., & Mack, A. (1992). Perception without attention: Results of a new method. Cognitive Psychology, g g 24, 502-534.
  43. Shomstein, S., & Yantis, S. (2002). Object-based attention: Sensory modulation or priority setting? Perception & Psychophysics, 64, 41-51.
  44. Treisman, A., Kahneman, D., & Burkell, J. (1983). Perceptual objects and the cost of filtering. Perception & Psychophysics, 33, 527-532.
  45. Treisman, A., & Schmidt, H. (1982). Illusory conjunctions in the per- ception of objects. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 107-141.
  46. Vecera, S. P. (1994). Grouped locations and object-based attention: Comment on Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 316-320.
  47. Vecera, S. P., & Farah, M. J. (1994). Does visual attention select ob- jects or locations? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 146-160.
  48. Watson, S. E., & Kramer, A. F. (1999). Object-based visual selective attention and perceptual organization. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 31-49.
  49. Yantis, S. (2000). Goal-directed and stimulus-directed determinants of attentional control (tutorial). In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 73-104). I Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.