Linguistic tools for embodied minds
2012, Rivista Italiana di Filosofia del Linguaggio
https://doi.org/10.4396/20120704Abstract
In this paper, we outline the embodied perspective of language comprehension indicating some of its limitations. We claim that the notions of language as a tool , might be useful to overcome a view focused only on referential aspects of language. Words, in quality of tools, can: a. facilitate communication among speakers; b. influence categorization; c. have a different impact on concrete and abstract words; d. allow us to construct an inner speech. Finally, we discuss how thinking about language as tool can help inform future research on thought, language and body.
References (78)
- AMEEL, E., STORMS, G., MALT, B. C. & SLOMAN, S. A. (2005), « How bilinguals solve the naming problem » in Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 60-
- AUSTIN, J. L. (1962), How to do things with words, Cambridge, Harvard University Press.
- BAK, T. H. & HODGES, J. R. (2004), « The effects of motor neurone disease on language: further evidence » in Brain and Language, 89, 2, 354-361.
- BARSALOU, L. W. & WIEMER-HASTINGS, K. (2005), « Situating abstract concepts" in PECHER, D. & ZWAAN, R. A. [Eds], Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in though, New York, Cambridge University Press.
- BARSALOU, L. W. (2008), « Grounded Cognition » in Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 1, 617-645.
- BLOOM, P. (1996), « Intention, history, and artifact concepts » in Cognition, 60, 1, 1-29.
- BORGHI, A. & CIMATTI, F. (2012), « Words are not just words: the social acquisition of abstract words » in Rivista Italiana di Filosofia del Linguaggio, 5, 22- 37. RIFL (2012) vol. 6, n.2: 43-58 DOI 10.4396/20120704 __________________________________________________________________________________
- BORGHI, A. M. & CIMATTI, F. (2009), « Words as tools and the problem of abstract words meanings » in TAATGEN, N. & VAN RIJN, H. [Eds], Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Amsterdam, Cognitive Science Society.
- BORGHI, A. M. & CIMATTI, F. (2010), « Embodied cognition and beyond: Acting and sensing the body » in Neuropsychologia, 48, 3, 763-773.
- BORGHI, A. M., FLUMINI, A., CIMATTI, F., MAROCCO, D. & SCOROLLI, C. (2011), « Manipulating objects and telling words: a study on concrete and abstract words acquisition » in Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 15.
- BORGHI, A. M. & PECHER, D. (2011), « Introduction to the special topic Embodied and Grounded Cognition » in Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 187.
- BORODITSKY, L. (2001), « Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers' conceptions of time » in Cognitive psychology, 43, 1 -22.
- BORODITSKY, L. (2003), « Linguistic Relativity » in NADEL, L. [Eds], Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science London, MacMillan Press.
- BOULENGER, V., MECHTOUFF, L., THOBOIS, S., BROUSSOLLE, E., JEANNEROD, M., et al. (2008), « Word processing in Parkinson's disease is impaired for action verbs but not for concrete nouns » in Neuropsychologia, 46, 2, 743-756.
- BOULENGER, V., SILBER, B. Y., ROY, A. C., PAULIGNAN, Y., JEANNEROD, M., et al. (2008), « Subliminal display of action words interferes with motor planning: a combined EEG and kinematic study » in Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102, 1-3, 130-136.
- BOYSEN, S. T., BERNSTON, G. G., HANNAN, M. B. & CACIOPPO, J. T. (1996), « Quantity-based interference and symbolic representations in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) » in Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes, 22, 1, 76-86.
- BUCCINO, G., RIGGIO, L., MELLI, G., BINKOFSKI, F., GALLESE, V., et al. (2005), « Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: A combined TMS and behavioral study » in Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 3, 355-363.
- CIMATTI, F. (1998), La scimmia che si parla. Linguaggio, autocoscienza e libertà nell'animale umano, Torino, Bollati e Boringhieri.
- CIMATTI, F. (2007), Il volto e la parola. Per una psicologia superficiale, Macerata, Quodlibet.
- CLARK, A. (1998), « Magic words: How language augments human computation » in CARRUTHERS, P. & BOUCHER, J. [Eds], Language and thought: Interdisciplinary themes, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
- CLARK, A. (2006a), « Material Symbols » in Philosophical Psychology, 19, 1-17.
- CLARK, A. (2006b), « Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche » in Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 8, 370-374.
- CLARK, H. H. & KRYCH, M. A. (2004), « Speaking while monitoring addressees for understanding » in Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 1, 62-81.
- DEACON, T. (1997), The symbolic species. The co-evolution of language and brain, New York, Norton.
- DEHAENE, S., SPELKE, E., PINEL, P., STANESCU, R. & TSIVKIN, S. (1999), « Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence » in Science, 284, 5416, 970-974.
- DESAI, R. H., BINDER, G. R., CONTANT, L. L. & SEIDENBERG, M. S. (2010), « Activation of sensory-motor areas in sentence comprehension » in Cerebral Cortex, 20, 468 -478.
- DOVE, G. (2009), « Beyond Conceptual Symbols. A Call for Representational Pluralism » in Cognition, 110, 412 -431.
- DOVE, G. (2011), « On the need for embodied and disembodied cognition » in Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 242.
- FASCHILLI, C. (2012), « La negazione e le teorie simulative della comprensione linguistica » in Rivista Italiana di Filosofia del Linguaggio, 5, 54-63.
- FERNYHOUGH, C. (2004), « Alien voices and inner dialogue: Towards a developmental account of auditory verbal hallucinations » in New Ideas in Psychology, 22, 49-68.
- FISCHER, M. H. & ZWAAN, R. A. (2008), « Embodied language: A review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension » in Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 6, 825-850.
- FODOR, J. (1983), The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology, Cambridge, MIT Press.
- GIANELLI, C., SCOROLLI, C. & BORGHI, A. (In Press), « Acting in perspective: the role of body and language as social tools » in Psychological Research, GLENBERG, A., SATO, M., CATTANEO, L., RIGGIO, L., PALUMBO, D., et al. (2008), « Processing abstract language modulates motor system activity » in The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 6, 905-919.
- GRICE, P. (1975), « Logic and conversation » in COLE, P. & MORGAN, J. L. [Eds], Syntax and Semantics, New York, Academic Press.
- HERMER-VAZQUEZ, L., SPELKE, E. S. & KATSNELSON, A. S. (1999), « Sources of flexibility in human cognition: dual-task studies of space and language » in Cognitive psychology, 39, 1, 3-36.
- HERRMANN, E., CALL, J., HERNANDEZ-LLOREDA, M. V., HARE, B. & TOMASELLO, M. (2007), « Humans Have Evolved Specialized Skills of Social Cognition: The Cultural Intelligence Hypothesis » in Science, 317, 5843, 1360-1366.
- JIRAK, D., MENZ, M. M., BUCCINO, G., BORGHI, A. M. & BINKOFSKI, F.
- « Grasping language -A short story on embodiment » in Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 3, 711-720.
- JONES, S. R. & FERNYHOUGH, C. (2007), « Neural correlates of inner speech and auditory verbal hallucinations: a critical review and theoretical integration » in Clinical psychology review, 27, 2, 140-154.
- LAKOFF, G. & JOHNSON, M. (1999), Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and Its Challenge to Western Thought, New York, Basic Books.
- LANDAU, M., SMITH, L. B. & JONES, S. S. (1988), « The importance of shape in early lexical learning » in Cognitive Development, 3, 229 -321.
- LI, P. & GLEITMAN, L. (2002), « Turning the tables: Language and spatial reasoning » in Cognition, 83, 265 -294.
- LIUZZA, M. T., CANDIDI, M. & AGLIOTI, S. M. (2011), « Do not resonate with actions: sentence polarity modulates cortico-spinal excitability during action-related sentence reading » in PLoS ONE, 6, 2, e16855.
- LIUZZA, M. T., CANDIDI, M. & S.M., A. (2012), « Non farlo! Negazione linguistica e simulazione delle azioni » in Rivista Italiana di Filosofia del Linguaggio, 5, 127-136.
- LUPYAN, G. (2012), « Linguistically modulated perception and cognition: the label- feedback hypothesis » in Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 54.
- MAHON, B. & CARAMAZZA, A. (2008), « A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content » in Journal of Physiology-Paris, 102, 1-3, 59-70.
- MALT, B. C., SLOMAN, S. A., GENNARI, S. P., SHI, M. & WANG, Y. (1999), « Knowing versus naming: Similarity and the linguistic categorization of artifact » in Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 230-262.
- MALT, B. C. & SLOMAN, S. A. (2004), « Conversation and convention: enduring influences on name choice for common objects » in Memory & cognition, 32, 8, 1346-1354.
- RIFL (2012) vol. 6, n.2: 43-58 DOI 10.4396/20120704 __________________________________________________________________________________ 56
- MALT, B. C. & SLOMAN, S. A. (2007), « Category essence or essentially pragmatic? Creator's intention in naming and what's really what » in Cognition, 105, 3, 615-648; discussion 649-657.
- MALT, B. C., GENNARI, S., IMAI, M., AMEEL, E., TSUDA, N., et al. (2008), « Talking about walking: Biomechanics and the language of locomotion » in Psychological Science, 19, 232 -240.
- MALT, B. C. e WOLFF, P. (eds.) (2010), Words and the mind: How words encode human experience, Oxford, Oxford University Press.
- MERLEAU-PONTY, M. (1945), Phénoménologie de la perception, Paris, Gallimard.
- MIROLLI, M. & PARISI, D. (2011), « Towards a Vygotskyan Cognitive Robotics: The role of language as a cognitive tool » in New Ideas in Psychology, 29, 3, 298- 311. MOLL, H. & TOMASELLO, M. (2007), « Cooperation and human cognition: the Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis » in Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological sciences, 362, 1480, 639-648.
- MURPHY, G. & MEDIN, D. (1985), « The Role of Theories in Conceptual Coherence » in Psychological Review, 92, 280-316.
- PECHER, D. & BOOT, T. (2011), « Numbers in space: differences between concrete and abstract situations » in Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 121.
- PECHER, D., BOOT, T. & VAN DANTZIG, S. (2011), « Abstract concepts: sensorymotor grounding, metaphors, and beyond » in ROSS, B. [Eds], The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Burlington, Academic Press.
- PEDERSON, E., WILKINS, D., LEVINSON, S. C., KITA, S. & SENFT, G. (1998), « Semantic typology and spatial conceptualization » in Language, 7, 3, 557 -589.
- PEIRCE, C. S. (1931 -1935), Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce, Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.
- PICKERING, M. J. & GARROD, S. (2004), « Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue » in The Behavioral and brain sciences, 27, 2, 169-190; discussion 190- 226. QUINE, W. V. (1960), Word and object, Cambridge, Technology Press of the Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
- REKERS, Y., HAUN, D. B. M. & TOMASELLO, M. (2011), « Children, but Not Chimpanzees, Prefer to Collaborate » in Current Biology, ROSH, E. (1978), « Principles of categorization » in ROSH, E. & LLOYD, B. B. [Eds], Cognition and Categorization, Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.
- ROSH, E. & MERVIS, C. B. (1978), « Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories » in Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573-605.
- SAUSSURE, F. (1916), Cours de linguistique générale, Paris, Payot.
- SCOROLLI, C., BINKOFSKI, F., BUCCINO, G., NICOLETTI, R., RIGGIO, L., et al. (2011), « Abstract and concrete sentences, embodiment, and languages » in Frontiers in Psychology, 227, SEARLE, J. R. (1969), Speech acts: an essay in the philosophy of language, Cambridge Eng., Cambridge University Press.
- SIMONDON, G. (1989), L'individuation psychique et collective, Paris, Auber.
- SPERBER, D. & WILSON, D. (1986), Relevance : communication and cognition, Cambridge, Mass., Harvard University Press.
- STEELS, L. (2003), « Language-reentrance and the Inner Voice » in Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10, 4-5, 173-185.
- TALMY, L. (1983), « How language structures space » in PICK, H. & ACREDOLO, L. [Eds], Spatial orientation: Theory, research and application, New York, Plenum Press.
- TAYLOR, L. & ZWAAN, R. (2008), « Motor resonance and linguistic focus » in The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 6, 896-904.
- TETTAMANTI, M., MANENTI, R., DELLA ROSA, P., FALINI, A., PERANI, D., et al. (2008), « Negation in the brain: Modulating action representations » in NeuroImage, 43, 2, 358-367.
- TOMASELLO, M., CARPENTER, M., CALL, J., BEHNE, T. & MOLL, H. (2005), « Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition » in The Behavioral and brain sciences, 28, 5, 675-691; discussion 691-735.
- TOMASELLO, M. (2009), Why We Cooperate, Cambridge, MIT Press.
- TOMASINO, B., WEISS, P. & FINK, G. (2010), « To move or not to move: imperatives modulate action-related verb processing in the motor system » in Neuroscience, TYLEN, K., WEED, E., WALLENTIN, M., ROEPSTORFF, A. & FRITH, C. D. (2010), « Language as a tool for interacting minds » in Mind and Language, 25, 3 -
- VYGOTSKY, L. (1934), Myš lenie i reč' Psichologičeskie issledovanija, Moskva- Leningrad, Gosudarstvennoe Social'no-Ekonome Izdatel'stvoičesko. RIFL (2012) vol. 6, n.2: 43-58 DOI 10.4396/20120704 __________________________________________________________________________________ 58
- WHORF, B. L. (1956), Language, thought, and reality; selected writings, Cambridge, Technology Press of Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
- WINAWER, J., WITTHOFT, N., FRANK, M. C., WU, L., WADE, A. R., et al. (2007), « Russian blues reveal effects of language on colour discrimination » in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 19, 7780-7785.
- WITTGENSTEIN, L. (1953), Philosophical investigations, New York,, Macmillan.
- ZWAAN, R. A., TAYLOR, L. J. & DE BOER, M. (2010), « Motor resonance as a function of narrative time: Further tests of the linguistic focus hypothesis » in Brain and Language, 112, 3, 143-149.