Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Outline

Distracted to a fault: Attention, actions, and time perception

Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics

https://doi.org/10.3758/S13414-022-02632-X

Abstract

In the last years, it has become general consensus that actions change our time perception. Performing an action to elicit a specific event seems to lead to a systematic underestimation of the interval between action and effect, a phenomenon termed temporal (or previously intentional) binding. Temporal binding has been closely associated with sense of agency, our perceived control over our actions and our environment, and because of its robust behavioral effects has indeed been widely utilized as an implicit correlate of sense of agency. The most robust and clear temporal binding effects are typically found via Libet clock paradigms. In the present study, we investigate a crucial methodological confound in these paradigms that provides an alternative explanation for temporal binding effects: a redirection of attentional resources in two-event sequences (as in classical operant conditions) versus singular events (as in classical baseline conditions). Our results indicate that binding...

References (54)

  1. Antusch, S., Aarts, H., & Custers, R. (2019). The role of intentional strength in shaping the sense of agency. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1124.
  2. Antusch, S., Custers, R., Marien, H., & Aarts, H. (2020). Intentionality and temporal binding: Do causality beliefs increase the perceived temporal attraction between events? Consciousness and Cognition, 77, 102835.
  3. Antusch, S., Custers, R., Marien, H., & Aarts, H. (2021). Intentional action and limitation of personal autonomy. Do restrictions of action selection decrease the sense of agency? Consciousness and Cognition, 88, 103076.
  4. Arrighi, R., Lunardi, R., & Burr, D. (2011). Vision and audition do not share attentional resources in sustained tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 56.
  5. Cheyne, J. A., Carriere, J. S., & Smilek, D. (2009). Absent minds and absent agents: Attention-lapse induced alienation of agency. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 481-493.
  6. Debats, N. B., Ernst, M. O., & Heuer, H. (2017). Perceptual attraction in tool use: evidence for a reliability-based weighting mechanism. Journal of Neurophysiology, 117(4), 1569-1580.
  7. Dewey, J. A., & Knoblich, G. (2014). Do implicit and explicit measures of the sense of agency measure the same thing? PLoS One, 9, e110118.
  8. Gable, P., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The blues broaden, but the nasty narrows: Attentional consequences of negative affects low and high in motivational intensity. Psychological Science, 21, 211-215.
  9. Golubickis, M., & Macrae, C. N. (2021). That's me in the spotlight: Self- relevance modulates attentional breadth. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28, 1915-1922.
  10. Goodhew, S. C., & Plummer, A. S. (2019). Flexibility in resizing atten- tional breadth: Asymmetrical versus symmetrical attentional con- traction and expansion costs depends on context. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 2527-2540.
  11. Haggard, P. (2017). Sense of agency in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18, 196-207.
  12. Haggard, P., & Tsakiris, M. (2009). The experience of agency: Feelings, judgments, and responsibility. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 242-246.
  13. Haggard, P., Clark, S., & Kalogeras, J. (2002). Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 382-385.
  14. Hoerl, C., Lorimer, S., McCormack, T., Lagnado, D. A., Blakey, E., Tecwyn, E. C., & Buehner, M. J. (2020). Temporal binding, causa- tion, and agency: Developing a new theoretical framework. Cognitive Science, 44, e12843.
  15. Hon, N. (2017). Attention and the sense of agency: A review and some thoughts on the matter. Consciousness and Cognition, 56, 30-36.
  16. Hon, N. (2022). Attention and expectation likely underlie temporal bind- ing measured using the Libet clock. Quarterly Journal of E x p e r i m e n t a l P s y c h o l o g y . h t t p s : / / d o i . o r g / 1 0 . 1 1 7 7 / 17470218221132762
  17. Humphreys, G. R., & Buehner, M. J. (2009). Magnitude estimation re- veals temporal binding at super-second intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1542.
  18. Imaizumi, S., & Tanno, Y. (2019). Intentional binding coincides with explicit sense of agency. Consciousness and Cognition, 67, 1-15.
  19. Isham, E. A., & Wall, T. A. (2022). Differentiating the reported time of intent and action on the basis of temporal binding behaviors and confidence ratings. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 84(4), 1328-1341.
  20. Ivanof, B. E., Terhune, D. B., Coyle, D., Gottero, M., & Moore, J. W. (2021). Examining the effect of Libet clock stimulus parameters on temporal binding. Psychological Research, 86, 937-951.
  21. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Prentice-Hall.
  22. Keitel, C., Maess, B., Schröger, E., & Müller, M. M. (2013). Early visual and auditory processing rely on modality-specific attentional re- sources. Neuroimage, 70, 240-249.
  23. Kirsch, W., & Kunde, W. (2021). The size of attentional focus modulates the perception of object location. Vision Research, 179, 1-8.
  24. Kirsch, W., Kunde, W., & Herbort, O. (2019). Intentional binding is unrelated to action intention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45, 378.
  25. Klaffehn, A. L., Sellmann, F. B., Kirsch, W., Kunde, W., & Pfister, R. (2021). Temporal binding as multisensory integration: Manipulating perceptual certainty of actions and their effects. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 83, 3135-3145.
  26. Kok, A., Ridderinkhof, K. R., & Ullsperger, M. (2006). The control of attention and actions: current research and future developments. Brain Research, 1105, 1-6.
  27. Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K. (1983). Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential): The unconscious initiation of a freely volun- tary act. Brain, 106, 623-642.
  28. Ling, S., & Carrasco, M. (2006). When sustained attention impairs per- ception. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 1243-1245.
  29. Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 476- 490. Majchrowicz, B., & Wierzchoń, M. (2018). Unexpected action outcomes produce enhanced temporal binding but diminished judgement of agency. Consciousness and Cognition, 65, 310-324.
  30. Matute, H., Cubillas, C. P., & Garaizar, P. (2017). Learning to infer the time of our actions and decisions from their consequences. Consciousness and Cognition, 56, 37-49.
  31. Moore, J. W. (2016). What is the sense of agency and why does it matter? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1272.
  32. Moore, J. W., Ruge, D., Wenke, D., Rothwell, J., & Haggard, P. (2010). Disrupting the experience of control in the human brain: pre- supplementary motor area contributes to the sense of agency. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277, 2503-2509.
  33. Muth, F. V., Wirth, R., & Kunde, W. (2022). Temporal binding in multi- step action-event sequences is driven by altered effect perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 99, 103299.
  34. Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2013). Confidence intervals for two sample means: Calculation, interpretation, and a few simple rules. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 9, 74.
  35. Pfister, R., Tonn, S., Weller, L., Kunde, W., & Schwarz, K. A. (2021). To prevent means to know: Explicit but no implicit agency for preven- tion behavior. Cognition, 206, 104489.
  36. Reddy, N. N. (2021). The implicit sense of agency is not a perceptual effect but is a judgment effect. Cognitive Processing, 23, 1-13.
  37. Ruess, M., Thomaschke, R., & Kiesel, A. (2017). The time course of intentional Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79, 1123-1131.
  38. Ruess, M., Thomaschke, R., Haering, C., Wenke, D., & Kiesel, A. (2018). Intentional binding of two effects. Psychological Research, 82, 1102-1112.
  39. Saito, N., Takahata, K., Murai, T., & Takahashi, H. (2015). Discrepancy between explicit judgement of agency and implicit feeling of agen- cy: Implications for sense of agency and its disorders. Consciousness and Cognition, 37, 1-7.
  40. Schaaf, M., Kunde, W., & Wirth, R. (2022). Monitoring goal-irrelevant effects interferes with concurrent tasks. Acta Psychologica, 224, 103522.
  41. Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic hu- man information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1-66.
  42. Schwarz, K. A., Weller, L., Klaffehn, A. L., & Pfister, R. (2019a, a). The effects of action choice on temporal binding, agency ratings, and their correlation. Consciousness and Cognition, 75, 102807.
  43. Schwarz, K. A., Weller, L., Pfister, R., & Kunde, W. (2019b, b). Connecting action control and agency: Does action-effect binding affect temporal binding?. Consciousness and Cognition, 76, 102833.
  44. Siebertz, M., & Jansen, P. (2022). Diverging implicit measurement of sense of agency using interval estimation and Libet clock. Consciousness and Cognition, 99, 103287.
  45. Suzuki, K., Lush, P., Seth, A. K., & Roseboom, W. (2019). Intentional binding without intentional action. Psychological Science, 30, 842- 853. Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., & Voss, M. (2013). The experience of agen- cy: an interplay between prediction and postdiction. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 127.
  46. Tanaka, T., Matsumoto, T., Hayashi, S., Takagi, S., & Kawabata, H. (2019). What makes action and outcome temporally close to each other: A systematic review and meta-analysis of temporal binding. Timing & Time Perception, 7, 189-218.
  47. Terashima, H., Kihara, K., Kawahara, J. I., & Kondo, H. M. (2021). Common principles underlie the fluctuation of auditory and visual sustained attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 705-715.
  48. Thomson, D. R., Besner, D., & Smilek, D. (2015). A resource-control account of sustained attention: Evidence from mind-wandering and vigilance paradigms. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 82-96.
  49. Tonn, S., Pfister, R., Klaffehn, A. L., Weller, L., & Schwarz, K. A. (2021). Two faces of temporal binding: Action-and effect-binding are not correlated. Consciousness and Cognition, 96, 103219.
  50. Tramacere, A., & Allen, C. (2022). Temporal binding: digging into ani- mal minds through time perception. Synthese, 200, 1-24.
  51. Venskus, A., Ferri, F., Migliorati, D., Spadone, S., Costantini, M., & Hughes, G. (2021). Temporal binding window and sense of agency are related processes modifiable via occipital tACS. PLoS One, 16, e0256987.
  52. Weller, L., Schwarz, K. A., Kunde, W., & Pfister, R. (2020). Something from nothing: Agency for deliberate nonactions. Cognition, 196, 104136.
  53. Wenke, D., Waszak, F., & Haggard, P. (2009). Action selection and action awareness. Psychological Research, 73(4), 602-612.
  54. Wirth, R., Janczyk, M., & Kunde, W. (2018). Effect monitoring in dual- task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 553-571.