Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Outline

Cognitive Neuroscience Meets the Community of Knowledge

2021, Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience

https://doi.org/10.3389/FNSYS.2021.675127

Abstract

Cognitive neuroscience seeks to discover the biological foundations of the human mind. One goal is to explain how mental operations are generated by the information processing architecture of the human brain. Our aim is to assess whether this is a well-defined objective. Our contention will be that it is not because the information processing of any given individual is not contained entirely within that individual’s brain. Rather, it typically includes components situated in the heads of others, in addition to being distributed across parts of the individual’s body and physical environment. Our focus here will be on cognition distributed across individuals, or on what we call the “community of knowledge,” the challenge that poses for reduction of cognition to neurobiology and the contribution of cognitive neuroscience to the study of communal processes.

References (91)

  1. Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 693-716. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707. 163514
  2. Anderson, E. (2006). The epistemology of democracy. Episteme 3, 8-22. doi: 10.1353/epi.0.0000
  3. Anduiza, E., Gallego, A., and Munoz, J. (2013). Turning a blind eye: experimental evidence of partisan bias in attitudes toward corruption. Comp. Political Stud. 46, 1664-1692. doi: 10.1177/0010414013489081
  4. Babiloni, F., and Astolfi, L. (2014). Social neuroscience and hyperscanning techniques: past, present and future. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 44, 76-93. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.07.006
  5. Bandettini, P. A. (2012). Twenty years of functional MRI: the science and the stories. Neuroimage 62, 575-588. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.026
  6. Banks, A. P., and Millward, L. J. (2000). Running shared mental models as a distributed cognitive process. Br. J. Psychol. 91(Pt 4), 513-531. doi: 10.1348/ 000712600161961
  7. Barbey, A. K., Karama, S., and Haier, R. J. (2021). Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence and Cognitive Neuroscience. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  8. Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). "Precursors to a theory of mind: understanding attention in others, " in Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading, Vol. 1, Ed. A. Whiten (Hoboken, NJ: Basil Blackwell), 233-251.
  9. Barsalou, L. W. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Mem. Cogn. 11, 211-227. doi: 10.3758/ bf03196968
  10. Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 617-645.
  11. Becchio, C., Sartori, L., and Castiello, U. (2010). Toward you: the social side of actions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 19, 183-188. doi: 10.1177/09637214103
  12. Boone, W., and Piccinini, G. (2016). The cognitive neuroscience revolution. Synthese 193, 1509â ȂŞ1534. doi: 10.1007/s11229-015-0783-4
  13. Boroditsky, L., and Gaby, A. (2010). Remembrances of times East: absolute spatial representations of time in an Australian aboriginal community. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1635-1639. doi: 10.1177/0956797610386621
  14. Bullock, J. G., Gerber, A. S., Hill, S. J., and Huber, G. A. (2015). Partisan bias in factual beliefs about politics. Q. J. Polit. Sci. 10, 519-578. doi: 10.1561/100. 00014074
  15. Burge, T. (1979). Individualism and the mental. Midwest Stud. Philos. 4, 73-121. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-4975.1979.tb00374.x
  16. Cacioppo, J. T., and Decety, J. (2011). An Introduction to Social Neuroscience. Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  17. Carroll, L. (1872). Through the Looking-Glass and What Alice Found There. Chicago: W.B. Conkey Co.
  18. Christakis, N. A., and Fowler, J. H. (2009). Connected: The Surprising Power of Our Social Networks and How They Shape Our Lives. New York, NY: Little, Brown Spark.
  19. Clark, A., and Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind (active externalism). Analysis 58, 7-19.
  20. Cohen, G. L. (2003). Party over policy: the dominating impact of group influence on political beliefs. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 808-822. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514. 85.5.808
  21. Cuc, A., Ozuru, Y., Manier, D., and Hirst, W. (2006). The transformation of collective memories: studies of family recounting. Mem. Cogn. 34, 752-762.
  22. Dennett, D. C. (2018). Facing up to the hard question of consciousness. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 373, 1-7.
  23. Dunning, D. (2011). The dunning-kruger effect: on being ignorant of one's own ignorance. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 44, 247-296. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12- 385522-0.00005-6
  24. Duque, J. F., Turner, R., Lewis, E. D., and Egan, G. (2010). Neuroanthropology: a humanistic science for the study of culture-brain nexus. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 5, 138-147. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp024
  25. Falk, E. B., and Bassett, D. S. (2017). Brain and social networks: fundamental building blocks of human experience. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 674-690. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.06.009
  26. Fernbach, P. M., Rogers, T., Fox, C. R., and Sloman, S. A. (2013). Political extremism is supported by an illusion of understanding. Psychol. Sci. 24, 939-946. doi: 10.1177/0956797612464058
  27. Fisher, A. J., Medaglia, J. D., and Jeronimus, B. F. (2018). Lack of group-to- individual generalizability is a threat to human subjects research. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 115, E6106-E6115.
  28. Fisher, M., Goddu, M. K., and Keil, F. C. (2015). Searching for explanations: how the internet inflates estimates of internal knowledge. J. Exp.l Psychol. Gen. 144, 674-687. doi: 10.1037/xge0000070
  29. Fisher, M., and Keil, F. C. (2014). The illusion of argument justification. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 425-433. doi: 10.1037/a0032234
  30. Flack, J. C. (2012). Multiple time-scales and the developmental dynamics of social systems. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 367, 1802-1810. doi: 10.1098/rstb. 2011.0214
  31. Frenda, S. J., Knowles, E. D., Saletan, W., and Loftus, E. F. (2013). False memories of fabricated political events. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 280-286. doi: 10.1016/j. jesp.2012.10.013
  32. Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., and Mangun, G. R. (2019). Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind. New York, NY: Norton & Company.
  33. Geana, M., Rabb, N., and Sloman, S. A. (2021). Walking the Party Line: The Growing Role of Political Ideology in Shaping Health Behavior in the United States. Manuscript under review
  34. Goldberg, S. (2016). The Twin Earth Chronicles: Twenty Years of Reflection on Hilary Putnam's the Meaning of Meaning: Twenty Years of Reflection on Hilary Putnam's the "Meaning of Meaning". London: Routledge.
  35. Goldman, A. I. (1999). Knowledge in a social world. Philos. Phenomenol. Res. 64, 185-190.
  36. Han, J. Y., and Federico, C. M. (2017). Conflict-framed news, self-categorization, and partisan polarization. Mass Commun. Soc. 20, 455-480. doi: 10.1080/ 15205436.2017.1292530
  37. Hasson, U., and Frith, C. D. (2016). Mirroring and beyond: coupled dynamics as a generalized framework for modelling social interactions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371:20150366. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0366
  38. Hawkins, C. B., and Nosek, B. A. (2012). Motivated independence? Implicit party identity predicts political judgments among self-proclaimed independents. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 38, 1437-1452. doi: 10.1177/0146167212452313
  39. Hayek, F. A. (1945). The use of knowledge in society. Am. Economic Rev. 35, 519-530.
  40. Hemmatian, B., Chan, S.-Y., and Sloman, S. A. (2019). "What gives a diagnostic label value? common use over informativeness, " in Poster at the Label entrenchment effect in explanation, Chicago, IL.
  41. Hemmatian, B., and Sloman, S. A. (2018). Community appeal: explanation without information. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 147, 1677-1712. doi: 10.1037/xge0000478
  42. Henrich, J. (2015). Culture and social behavior. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 3, 84-89.
  43. Henrich, N., and Henrich, J. (2007). Why Human Cooperate: A Cultural and Evolutionary Explanation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  44. Hirst, W., and Manier, D. (1996). "Social influences on remembering, " in Remembering the Past, ed. D. Rubin (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 271-290. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511527913.011
  45. Holmes, R. M. (2020). Cultural Psychology: Exploring Culture and Mind in Diverse Communities. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/oso/ 9780199343805.001.0001
  46. Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. doi: 10. 7551/mitpress/1881.001.0001
  47. Jacobson, G. C. (2010). Perception, memory, and partisan polarization on the Iraq war. Polit. Sci. Q. 125, 31-56. doi: 10.1002/j.1538-165x.2010.tb00667.x
  48. Kahan, D. M., Jenkins-Smith, H., and Braman, D. (2011). Cultural cognition of scientific consensus. J. Risk Res. 14, 147-174. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2010. 511246
  49. Kominsky, J. F., and Keil, F. C. (2014). Overestimation of knowledge about word meanings: the "misplaced meaning". Effect. Cogn. Sci. 38, 1604-1633. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12122
  50. Lawson, R. (2006). The science of cycology: failures to understand how everyday objects work. Mem. Cognit. 34, 1667-1675. doi: 10.3758/bf03195929
  51. Marks, J., Copland, E., Loh, E., Sunstein, C. R., and Sharot, T. (2019). Epistemic spillovers: learning others' political views reduces the ability to assess and use their expertise in nonpolitical domains. Cognition 188, 74-84. doi: 10.1016/j. cognition.2018.10.003
  52. Marr, D. (1982). Vision : A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information. San Francisco, CA: W.H. Freeman.
  53. Meade, M. L., Harris, C. B., Van Bergen, P., Sutton, J., and Barnier, A. J. (2018). Collaborative Remembering. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/oso/ 9780198737865.001.0001
  54. Mercier, H., and Sperber, D. (2017). The Enigma of Reason. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  55. Montague, P. R., Berns, G. S., Cohen, J. D., McClure, S. M., Pagnoni, G., Dhamala, M., et al. (2002). Hyperscanning: simultaneous fMRI during linked social interactions. Neuroimage 16, 1159-1164. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1150
  56. Moreau, Q., and Dumas, G. (2021). Beyond correlation versus causation: multi- brain neuroscience needs explanation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 542-543. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.02.011
  57. Nickerson, R. S. (1999). How we know -and sometimes misjudge -what others know: imputing one's own knowledge to others. Psychol. Bull. 125, 737-759. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.737
  58. Novembre, G., and Iannetti, G. (2020). Proving causality in hyperscanning: multibrain stimulation and other approaches: response to Moreau and Dumas. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 544-545. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.03.013
  59. Ober, J. (2008). Democracy and Knowledge: Innovation and Learning in Classical Athens. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. doi: 10.1515/97814008 28807
  60. Parkinson, C., Kleinbaum, A. M., and Wheatley, T. (2017). Spontaneous neural encoding of social network position. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1:0072. doi: 10.1038/ s41562-017-0072
  61. Paulhus, D. L., Harms, P. D., Bruce, M. N., and Lysy, D. C. (2003). The over- claiming technique: measuring self-enhancement independent of ability. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 890-904. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.890
  62. Pentland, A. S. (2012). The new science of building great teams. Harv. Bus. Rev. 90, 60-69.
  63. Putnam, H. (1975). Philosophical Papers. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  64. Rabb, N., Fernbach, P., and Sloman, S. A. (2019). Individual representation in a community of knowledge. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 891-902. doi: 10.1016/j.tics. 2019.07.011
  65. Richerson, P. J., Boyd, R., and Henrich, J. (2010). Gene-culture coevolution in the age of genomics. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 107(Suppl. 2), 8985-8992. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0914631107
  66. Rilling, J. K., and Sanfey, A. G. (2011). The neuroscience of social decision-making. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 62, 23-48. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131647
  67. Rosch, E. (1978). "Principles of categorization, " in Cognition and Categorization, eds E. Rosch, and B. B. Lloyd (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 28-49.
  68. Rozenblit, L., and Keil, F. (2002). The misunderstood limits of folk science: an illusion of explanatory depth. Cogn. Sci. 26, 521-562. doi: 10.1207/ s15516709cog2605_1
  69. Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T., et al. (2013). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 393-414. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x12000660
  70. Schmalzle, R., Hacker, F. E., Honey, C. J., and Hasson, U. (2015). Engaged listeners: shared neural processing of powerful political speeches. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 1137-1143. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu168
  71. Searle, J. R. (2000). Consciousness. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 557-578. doi: 10.1146/ annurev.neuro.23.1.557
  72. Servick, K. (2020). In two-person MRI, brains socialize at close range. Science 367:133. doi: 10.1126/science.367.6474.133
  73. Silbert, L. J., Honey, C. J., Simony, E., Poeppel, D., and Hasson, U. (2014). Coupled neural systems underlie the production and comprehension of naturalistic narrative speech. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 111, E4687-E4696. doi: 10.1073/ pnas.1323812111
  74. Simony, E., Honey, C. J., Chen, J., Lositsky, O., Yeshurun, Y., Wiesel, A., et al. (2016). Dynamic reconfiguration of the default mode network during narrative comprehension. Nat. Commun. 7:12141. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12141
  75. Sloman, S. A., and Fernbach, P. (2017). The Knowledge Illusion : Why we Never Think Alone. New York, NY: Riverhead Books.
  76. Sloman, S. A., and Rabb, N. (2016). Your understanding is my understanding: evidence for a community of knowledge. Psychol. Sci. 27, 1451-1460. doi: 10.1177/0956797616662271
  77. Stephens, G. J., Silbert, L. J., and Hasson, U. (2010). Speaker-listener neural coupling underlies successful communication. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107, 14425-14430. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008662107
  78. Theiner, G. (2013). Transactive Memory Systems: A Mechanistic Analysis of Emergent Group Memory. Rev. Philos. Psychol. 4, 65-89. doi: 10.1007/s13164- 012-0128-x
  79. Tomasello, M., and Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Dev. Sci. 10, 121-125.
  80. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., and Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 675-735. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x05000129
  81. Toppi, J., Ciaramidaro, A., Vogel, P., Mattia, D., Babiloni, F., Siniatchkin, M., et al. (2015). "Graph theory in brain-to-brain connectivity: a simulation study and an application to an EEG hyperscanning experiment, " in Proceedings of the 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), Burlington, MA, 2211-2214. doi: 10.1109/EMBC.2015. 7318830
  82. Turner, B. O., Santander, T., Paul, E. J., Barbey, A. K., and Miller, M. B. (2019). Reply to: fMRI replicability depends upon sufficient individual-level data. Commun. Biol. 2:129. doi: 10.1038/s42003-019-0379-5
  83. Van Boven, L., Ehret, P. J., and Sherman, D. K. (2018). Psychological Barriers to bipartisan public support for climate policy. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 13, 492-507. doi: 10.1177/1745691617748966
  84. Vitriol, J. A., and Marsh, J. K. (2018). The illusion of explanatory depth and endorsement of conspiracy beliefs. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 48, 955-969. doi: 10. 1002/ejsp.2504
  85. Weatherford, J. (2005). Genghis Khan and the Making of the Modern World. Broadway Books. New York, NY: Crown and Three Rivers Press.
  86. Wegner, D. M., Erber, R., and Raymond, P. (1991). Transactive memory in close relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61, 923-929. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61. 6.923
  87. Wilson, R. A. (2005). Collective memory, group minds, and the extended mind thesis. Cogn. Process 6, 227-236. doi: 10.1007/s10339-005-0012-z
  88. Wilson, R. A., and Keil, F. (1998). The shadows and shallows of explanation. Minds Mach. 8, 137-159. doi: 10.1023/A:1008259020140
  89. Wittgenstein, L. (1973). Philosophical Investigations, 3rd Edn. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
  90. Woolley, A. W., Chabris, C. F., Pentland, A., Hashmi, N., and Malone, T. W. (2010). Evidence for a collective intelligence factor in the performance of human groups. Science 330, 686-688. doi: 10.1126/science.11
  91. Zaller, J. (1992). The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511818691