Theories and methods in the scientific study of consciousness
2015, Advances in Consciousness Research
https://doi.org/10.1075/AICR.92.02KLIAbstract
The scientific study of consciousness has gained much interest over the past several decades. Here, we provide a primer on the topic by introducing the most commonly used concepts, the most prominent scientific theories, and an overview of broadly used methods of studying consciousness. With a focus on visual perception, we discuss the distinction between phenomenology and cognitive accessibility in the context of the different consciousness theories and highlight elements in which these theories are either complementary or more or less in agreement.
References (142)
- Arnold, D. H., Clifford, C. W., & Wenderoth, P. (2001). Asynchronous processing in vision: Color leads motion. Current Biology, 11(8), 596-600. DOI: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00156-7
- Azzopardi, P., & Cowey, A. (1997). Is blindsight like normal, near-threshold vision? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 94(25), 14190-14194. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.94.25.14190
- Baars, B. J. (1993). A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
- Baars, B. J. (1997). In the theater of consciousness: The workspace of the mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195102659.001.1
- Blake, R., & Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Visual competition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(1), 13-21. DOI: 10.1038/nrn701
- Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18(2), 227-287. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X00038188
- Block, N. (1996). How can we find the neural correlate of consciousness? Trends in Neuro- sciences, 19(11), 456-459. DOI: 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)20049-9
- Block, N. (2005a). Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 46-52. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.006
- Block, N. (2005b). The merely verbal problem of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(6), 270. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.009
- Block, N. (2011). Perceptual consciousness overflows cognitive access. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(12), 567-575. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.001
- Bonneh, Y. S., Cooperman, A., & Sagi, D. (2001). Motion-induced blindness in normal observ- ers. Nature, 411(6839), 798-801. DOI: 10.1038/35081073
- Born, R. T., & Bradley, D. C. (2005). Structure and function of visual area MT. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 157-189. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131052
- Bosman, C. A., Schoffelen, J.-M., Brunet, N., Oostenveld, R., Bastos, A. M., Womelsdorf, T., Rubehn, B., Stieglitz, T., De Weerd, P., & Fries, P. (2012). Attentional stimulus selection through selective synchronization between monkey visual areas. Neuron, 75(5), 875-888. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.037
- Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesensten, N. J., Gwadry, F., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., Baldwin, P., Belenky, G., & Herscovitch, P. (1998). Dissociated pattern of activity in visual cortices and their projections during human rapid eye movement sleep. Science, 279(5347), 91-95. DOI: 10.1126/science.279.5347.91
- Broadbent, D. E., & Broadbent, M. H. (1987). From detection to identification: Response to mul- tiple targets in rapid serial visual presentation. Perception & Psychophysics, 42(2), 105-113. DOI: 10.3758/BF03210498
- Carrasco, M., Ling, S., & Read, S. (2004). Attention alters appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 7(3), 308-313. DOI: 10.1038/nn1194
- Chalmers, D. J. (1995). Facing up to the problem of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2(3), 200-219.
- Chalmers, D. J. (1996). The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
- Cohen, M. A., Cavanagh, P., Chun, M. M., & Nakayama, K. (2012). The attentional requirements of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(8), 411-417. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.013
- Cohen, M. A., & Dennett, D. C. (2011). Consciousness cannot be separated from function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(8), 358-364. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.06.008
- Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1990). Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Seminars in the Neurosciences, 2, 263-275. DOI: 10.1016/B978-0-12-185254-2.50021-8
- Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1995). Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex? Nature, 375(6527), 121-123. DOI: 10.1038/375121a0
- Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1998). Consciousness and neuroscience. Cerebral Cortex, 8(2), 97-107. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/8.2.97
- Crick, F., & Koch, C. (2003). A framework for consciousness. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 119- 126. DOI: 10.1038/nn0203-119
- Dehaene, S., Changeux, J.-P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, precon- scious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204-211. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007
- Dehaene, S., Kerszberg, M., & Changeux, J.-P. (1998). A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 95(24), 14529-14534. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.95.24.14529
- Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79(1-2), 1-37. DOI: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00123-2
- Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., Bihan, D. L., Mangin, J. F., Poline, J. B., & Rivière, D. (2001). Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nature Neu- roscience, 4(7), 752-758. DOI: 10.1038/89551
- Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec'H, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., van de Moortele, P. F., & Le Bihan, D. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395(6702), 597-600. DOI: 10.1038/26967
- Driver, J., & Mattingley, J. B. (1998). Parietal neglect and visual awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 1(1), 17-22. DOI: 10.1038/217
- Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural darwinism: The theory of neuronal group selection. New York, NY: Basic Books.
- Edelman, G. M. (1993). Neural darwinism: Selection and reentrant signaling in higher brain function. Neuron, 10(2), 115-125. DOI: 10.1016/0896-6273(93)90304-A
- Edelman, G. M., Gally, J. A., & Baars, B. J. (2011). Biology of consciousness. Frontiers in Psychol- ogy, 2, 4. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00004
- Edelman, G. M., & Mountcastle, V. B. (1978). The mindful brain: Cortical organization and the group-selective theory of higher brain function. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Edelman, G. M., & Tononi, G. (2000). A universe of consciousness: How matter becomes imagina- tion. New York, NY: Basic Books.
- Fahrenfort, J. J., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2012). A true science of consciousness explains phenom- enology: Comment on Cohen and Dennett. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 138-139. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.004
- Fahrenfort, J. J., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2007). Masking disrupts reentrant processing in human visual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 1488-1497. DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1488
- Felleman, D., & Van Essen, D. C. (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 1(1), 1-47. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1
- Ferrera, V. P., Rudolph, K. K., & Maunsell, J. H. (1994). Responses of neurons in the parietal and temporal visual pathways during a motion task. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(10), 6171-6186.
- Gaillard, R., Dehaene, S., Adam, C., Clémenceau, S., Hasboun, D., Baulac, M., Cohen, L., & Nacchache, L. (2009). Converging intracranial markers of conscious access. PLoS Biology, 7(3), e61. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000061
- Galvin, S. J., Podd, J. V, Drga, V., & Whitmore, J. (2003). Type 2 tasks in the theory of signal detectability: Discrimination between correct and incorrect decisions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(4), 843-876. DOI: 10.3758/BF03196546
- Goodale, M. A. (2004). An evolving view of duplex vision: Separate but interacting cortical pathways for perception and action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(2), 203-211. DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.002
- Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15(1), 20-25. DOI: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8
- Goodale, M. A., Milner, A. D., Jakobson, L. S., & Carey, D. P. (1991). A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them. Nature, 349(6305), 154-156. DOI: 10.1038/349154a0
- Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S. C., & Abrams, R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 273(5282), 1699-1702. DOI: 10.1126/science.273.5282.1699
- Grill-Spector, K., & Malach, R. (2004). The human visual cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 649-677. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144220
- Hameroff, S. (2006a). The entwined mysteries of anesthesia and consciousness: Is there a com- mon underlying mechanism? Anesthesiology, 105(2), 400-412. DOI: 10.1097/00000542-200608000-00024
- Hameroff, S. (2006b). Consciousness, neurobiology and quantum mechanics: The case for a connection. In J. A. Tuszynski (Ed.), The emerging physics of consciousness (pp. 193-253). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. DOI: 10.1007/3-540-36723-3_6
- Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top- down attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3), 284-291. DOI: 10.1038/72999
- Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(1), 12-18. DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00013-X
- Lamme, V. A. F. (2004). Separate neural definitions of visual consciousness and visual attention: A case for phenomenal awareness. Neural Networks, 17(5-6), 861-872. DOI: 10.1016/j.neunet. 2004.02.005
- Lamme, V. A. F. (2006). Towards a true neural stance on consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(11), 494-501. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.001
- Lamme, V. A. F. (2010). How neuroscience will change our view on consciousness. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(3), 204-220. DOI: 10.1080/17588921003731586
- Lamme, V. A. F., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 23(11), 571-579. DOI: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01657-X
- Lamme, V. A. F., Supèr, H., & Spekreijse, H. (1998). Feedforward, horizontal, and feedback pro- cessing in the visual cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8(4), 529-535. DOI: 10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80042-1
- Lamme, V. A. F., Zipser, K., & Spekreijse, H. (2002). Masking interrupts figure-ground signals in V1. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(7), 1044-1053. DOI: 10.1162/089892902320474490
- Landman, R., Spekreijse, H., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness. Vision Research, 43(2), 149-164. DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00402-9
- Levi, D. M. (2008). Crowding -An essential bottleneck for object recognition: A mini-review. Vision Research, 48(5), 635-654. DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.12.009
- Levine, J. (1983). Materialism and qualia: The explanatory gap. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly, 64(4), 354-361.
- Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and con- junctions. Nature, 390(6657), 279-281. DOI: 10.1038/36846
- Mack, A. (2003). Inattentional blindness: Looking without seeing. Current Directions in Psycho- logical Science, 12(5), 180-184. DOI: 10.1111/1467-8721.01256
- Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- Macknik, S. L. (2006). Visual masking approaches to visual awareness. Progress in Brain Research, 155, 177-215. DOI: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)55011-3
- Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, D. C. (1991). Detection theory: A user's guide. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
- Maniscalco, B., & Lau, H. (2012). A signal detection theoretic approach for estimating meta- cognitive sensitivity from confidence ratings. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 422-430. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.09.021
- Marcel, A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15(2), 197-237. DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(83)90009-9
- Marchetti, G. (2012). Against the view that consciousness and attention are fully dissociable. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 36. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00036
- Martens, S., & Wyble, B. (2010). The attentional blink: Past, present, and future of a blind spot in perceptual awareness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(6), 947-957. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.005
- Maunsell, J. H. R., & Newsome, W. T. (1987). Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 10, 363-401. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.ne.10.030187.002051
- McFadden, J. (2002a). Synchronous firing and its influence on the brains electromagnetic field. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9(4), 23-50.
- McFadden, J. (2002b). The conscious electromagnetic information (Cemi) field theory: The hard problem made easy? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9(8), 45-60.
- Merigan, W. H., & Maunsell, J. H. (1993). How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 369-402. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.002101
- Merikle, P., Smilek, D., & Eastwood, J. (2001). Perception without awareness: Perspectives from cognitive psychology. Cognition, 79(1-2), 115-134. DOI: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00126-8
- Mesulam, M. M. (1998). From sensation to cognition. Brain, 121(6), 1013-1052. DOI: 10.1093/brain/121.6.1013
- Miller, S. M. (2001). Binocular rivalry and the cerebral hemispheres: With a note on the cor- relates and constitution of visual consciousness. Brain and Mind, 2(1), 119-149. DOI: 10.1023/A:1017981619014
- Miller, S. M. (2007). On the correlation/constitution distinction problem (and other hard prob- lems) in the scientific study of consciousness. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 19(3), 159-176. DOI: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2007.00207.x
- Miller, S. M. (Ed.). (2013). The constitution of visual consciousness: Lessons from binocular rivalry. Advances in Consciousness Research (Vol. 90). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company. DOI: 10.1075/aicr.90
- Milner, A., & Goodale, M. A. (1993). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Pro- gress in Brain Research, 95, 317-337. DOI: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60379-9
- Mishkin, M., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1982). Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial func- tions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys. Behavioural Brain Research, 6(1), 57-77. DOI: 10.1016/0166-4328(82)90081-X
- Moutoussis, K., & Zeki, S. (1997a). A direct demonstration of perceptual asynchrony in vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 264(1380), 393-399. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0056
- Moutoussis, K., & Zeki, S. (1997b). Functional segregation and temporal hierarchy of the vis- ual perceptive systems. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 264(1387), 1407-1414. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0196
- Moutoussis, K., & Zeki, S. (2002). The relationship between cortical activation and perception investigated with invisible stimuli. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99(14), 9527-9532. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.142305699
- Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review, 83(4), 435-450. DOI: 10.2307/2183914
- Nakamura, K., Dehaene, S., Jobert, A., Le Bihan, D., & Kouider, S. (2007). Task-specific change of unconscious neural priming in the cerebral language network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104(49), 19643-19648. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0704487104
- Oizumi, M., Albantakis, L., & Tononi, G. (2014). From the phenomenology to the mechanisms of consciousness: Integrated Information Theory 3.0. PLoS Computational Biology, 10(5), e1003588. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003588
- O'Regan, J. K., Rensink, R. A., & Clark, J. J. (1999). Change-blindness as a result of "mud- splashes". Nature, 398(6722), 34. DOI: 10.1038/17953
- Owen, A. M., & Coleman, M. R. (2008). Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1129, 130-138. DOI: 10.1196/annals.1417.018
- Owen, A. M., Coleman, M. R., Boly, M., Davis, M. H., Laureys, S., & Pickard, J. D. (2006). Detect- ing awareness in the vegetative state. Science, 313(5792), 1402. DOI: 10.1126/science.1130197
- Pelli, D. G., Palomares, M., & Majaj, N. J. (2004). Crowding is unlike ordinary masking: Distin- guishing feature integration from detection. Journal of Vision, 4(12), 12. DOI: 10.1167/4.12.12
- Pelli, D. G., & Tillman, K. A. (2008). The uncrowded window of object recognition. Nature Neu- roscience, 11(10), 1129-1135. DOI: 10.1038/nn.2187
- Persaud, N., McLeod, P., & Cowey, A. (2007). Post-decision wagering objectively measures awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 10(2), 257-261. DOI: 10.1038/nn1840
- Pinto, Y., Sligte, I. G., Shapiro, K. L., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2013). Fragile visual short-term mem- ory is an object-based and location-specific store. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(4), 732-739. DOI: 10.3758/s13423-013-0393-4
- Pöppel, E., Brinkmann, R., Von Cramon, D., & Singer, W. (1978). Association and dissociation of visual functions in a case of bilateral occipital lobe infarction. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 225(1), 1-21. DOI: 10.1007/BF00367348
- Pöppel, E., Held, R., & Frost, D. (1973). Residual visual function after brain wounds involving the central visual pathways in man. Nature, 243(5405), 295-296. DOI: 10.1038/243295a0
- Posner, M. I. (1994). Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 91(16), 7398-7403. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.91.16.7398
- Posner, M. I. (2012). Attentional networks and consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 64. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00064
- Potter, M. C., Chun, M. M., Banks, B. S., & Muckenhoupt, M. (1998). Two attentional deficits in serial target search: The visual attentional blink and an amodal task-switch deficit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(4), 979-992. DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.24.4.979
- Price, N. S. C. (2013). Overview of visual system structure and function. In S. M. Miller (Ed.), The constitution of visual consciousness: Lessons from binocular rivalry (pp. 37-76). Advances in Consciousness Research (Vol. 90). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publish- ing Company. DOI: 10.1075/aicr.90.03pri
- Raffone, A., & Pantani, M. (2010). A global workspace model for phenomenal and access con- sciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(2), 580-596. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.013
- Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual process- ing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 849-860. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.849
- Rees, G., Kreiman, G., & Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(4), 261-270. DOI: 10.1038/nrn783
- Rees, G., Wojciulik, E., Clarke, K., Husain, M., Frith, C., & Driver, J. (2000). Unconscious activa- tion of visual cortex in the damaged right hemisphere of a parietal patient with extinction. Brain, 123(8), 1624-1633. DOI: 10.1093/brain/123.8.1624
- Rees, G., Wojciulik, E., Clarke, K., Husain, M., Frith, C., & Driver, J. (2002). Neural correlates of conscious and unconscious vision in parietal extinction. Neurocase, 8(5), 387-393. DOI: 10.1093/neucas/8.5.387
- Rensink, R. A., O'Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8(5), 368-373. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00427.x
- Revonsuo, A. (2001). Can functional brain imaging discover consciousness in the brain? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(3), 3-23.
- Roe, A. W., Chelazzi, L., Connor, C. E., Conway, B. R., Fujita, I., Gallant, J. L., Lu, H., & Vanduffel, W. (2012). Toward a unified theory of visual area V4. Neuron, 74(1), 12-29. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.011
- Roelfsema, P. R. (2006). Cortical algorithms for perceptual grouping. Annual Review of Neurosci- ence, 29, 203-227. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112939
- Seidemann, E., Poirson, A. B., Wandell, B. A., & Newsome, W. T. (1999). Color signals in area MT of the macaque monkey. Neuron, 24(4), 911-917. DOI: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81038-7
- Seth, A. K. (2009). Explanatory correlates of consciousness: Theoretical and computational chal- lenges. Cognitive Computation, 1(1), 50-63. DOI: 10.1007/s12559-009-9007-x
- Seth, A. K. (2010). The grand challenge of consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 5. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00005
- Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28(9), 1059-1074. DOI: 10.1068/p2952
- Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (1997). Change blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(7), 261- 267. DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01080-2
- Simons, D. J., & Rensink, R. A. (2005). Change blindness: Past, present, and future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(1), 16-20. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.006
- Sligte, I. G., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2008). Are there multiple visual short-term mem- ory stores? PLoS ONE, 3(2), e1699. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001699
- Sligte, I. G., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2009). V4 activity predicts the strength of visual short-term memory representations. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(23), 7432-7438. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0784-09.2009
- Sligte, I. G., Wokke, M. E., Tesselaar, J. P., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2010). Magnetic stim- ulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dissociates fragile visual short-term memory from visual working memory. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1578-1588. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.010
- Sperling, G. (1960). Negative afterimage without prior positive image. Science, 131(3413), 1613- 1614. DOI: 10.1126/science.131.3413.1613
- Stoerig, P., & Cowey, A. (2007). Blindsight. Current Biology, 17(19), R822-R824. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.016
- Tononi, G. (2004). An information integration theory of consciousness. BMC Neuroscience, 5, 42. DOI: 10.1186/1471-2202-5-42
- Tononi, G. (2008). Consciousness as integrated information: A provisional manifesto. Biological Bulletin, 215(3), 216-242. DOI: 10.2307/25470707
- Tononi, G., & Edelman, G. M. (1998). Consciousness and complexity. Science, 282(5395), 1846- 1851. DOI: 10.1126/science.282.5395.1846
- Tononi, G., & Koch, C. (2008). The neural correlates of consciousness: An update. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 239-261. DOI: 10.1196/annals.1440.004
- Tononi, G., & Sporns, O. (2003). Measuring information integration. BMC Neuroscience, 4, 31. DOI: 10.1186/1471-2202-4-31
- Tovee, M. J. (1994). How fast is the speed of thought? Current Biology, 4(12), 1125-1127. DOI: 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00253-0
- Treue, S. (2003). Visual attention: The where, what, how and why of saliency. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13(4), 428-432. DOI: 10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00105-3
- Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1096-1101. DOI: 10.1038/nn1500
- van Boxtel, J. J. A., Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2010a). Consciousness and attention: On suffi- ciency and necessity. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 217. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00217
- van Boxtel, J. J. A., Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2010b). Opposing effects of attention and con- sciousness on afterimages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107(19), 8883-8888. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0913292107
- Vandenbroucke, A. R., Sligte, I. G., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2011). Manipulations of attention dis- sociate fragile visual short-term memory from visual working memory. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1559-1568. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.044
- Wallis, T. S. A., & Bex, P. J. (2011). Visual crowding is correlated with awareness. Current Biology, 21(3), 254-258. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.01.011
- Watanabe, M., Cheng, K., Murayama, Y., Ueno, K., Asamizuya, T., Tanaka, K., & Logothetis, N. (2011). Attention but not awareness modulates the BOLD signal in the human V1 during binocular suppression. Science, 334(6057), 829-831. DOI: 10.1126/science.1203161
- Weiskrantz, L., Barbur, J. L., & Sahraie, A. (1995). Parameters affecting conscious versus uncon- scious visual discrimination with damage to the visual cortex (V1). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 92(13), 6122-6126. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.92.13.6122
- Weiskrantz, L., Warrington, E. K., Sanders, M. D., & Marshall, J. (1974). Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. Brain, 97(4), 709-728. DOI: 10.1093/brain/97.1.709
- Whitney, D., & Levi, D. M. (2011). Visual crowding: A fundamental limit on conscious percep- tion and object recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(4), 160-168. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.02.005
- Wilke, M., Logothetis, N. K., & Leopold, D. A. (2003). Generalized flash suppression of salient visual targets. Neuron, 39(6), 1043-1052. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2003.08.003
- Wolfe, J. M. (1984). Reversing ocular dominance and suppression in a single flash. Vision Research, 24(5), 471-478. DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(84)90044-0
- Zeki, S. (2001). Localization and globalization in conscious vision. Annual Review of Neurosci- ence, 24, 57-86. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.57
- Zeki, S. (2008). The disunity of consciousness. Progress in Brain Research, 168, 11-18. DOI: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)68002-9
- Zeki, S., & Bartels, A. (1998a). The autonomy of the visual systems and the modularity of conscious vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 353(1377), 1911-1914. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0343
- Zeki, S., & Bartels, A. (1998b). The asynchrony of consciousness. Proceedings of the Royal Soci- ety of London B: Biological Sciences, 265(1405), 1583-1585. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.1998.0475
- Zeki, S., & ffytche, D. H. (1998). The Riddoch syndrome: Insights into the neurobiology of con- scious vision. Brain, 121(1), 25-45. DOI: 10.1093/brain/121.1.25
- Zipser, K., Lamme, V. A. F., & Schiller, P. H. (1996). Contextual modulation in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 16(22), 7376-7389.