Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Outline

Theories and methods in the scientific study of consciousness

2015, Advances in Consciousness Research

https://doi.org/10.1075/AICR.92.02KLI

Abstract

The scientific study of consciousness has gained much interest over the past several decades. Here, we provide a primer on the topic by introducing the most commonly used concepts, the most prominent scientific theories, and an overview of broadly used methods of studying consciousness. With a focus on visual perception, we discuss the distinction between phenomenology and cognitive accessibility in the context of the different consciousness theories and highlight elements in which these theories are either complementary or more or less in agreement.

References (142)

  1. Arnold, D. H., Clifford, C. W., & Wenderoth, P. (2001). Asynchronous processing in vision: Color leads motion. Current Biology, 11(8), 596-600. DOI: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00156-7
  2. Azzopardi, P., & Cowey, A. (1997). Is blindsight like normal, near-threshold vision? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 94(25), 14190-14194. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.94.25.14190
  3. Baars, B. J. (1993). A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  4. Baars, B. J. (1997). In the theater of consciousness: The workspace of the mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195102659.001.1
  5. Blake, R., & Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Visual competition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(1), 13-21. DOI: 10.1038/nrn701
  6. Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18(2), 227-287. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X00038188
  7. Block, N. (1996). How can we find the neural correlate of consciousness? Trends in Neuro- sciences, 19(11), 456-459. DOI: 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)20049-9
  8. Block, N. (2005a). Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 46-52. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.006
  9. Block, N. (2005b). The merely verbal problem of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(6), 270. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.009
  10. Block, N. (2011). Perceptual consciousness overflows cognitive access. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(12), 567-575. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.001
  11. Bonneh, Y. S., Cooperman, A., & Sagi, D. (2001). Motion-induced blindness in normal observ- ers. Nature, 411(6839), 798-801. DOI: 10.1038/35081073
  12. Born, R. T., & Bradley, D. C. (2005). Structure and function of visual area MT. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 157-189. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131052
  13. Bosman, C. A., Schoffelen, J.-M., Brunet, N., Oostenveld, R., Bastos, A. M., Womelsdorf, T., Rubehn, B., Stieglitz, T., De Weerd, P., & Fries, P. (2012). Attentional stimulus selection through selective synchronization between monkey visual areas. Neuron, 75(5), 875-888. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.037
  14. Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesensten, N. J., Gwadry, F., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., Baldwin, P., Belenky, G., & Herscovitch, P. (1998). Dissociated pattern of activity in visual cortices and their projections during human rapid eye movement sleep. Science, 279(5347), 91-95. DOI: 10.1126/science.279.5347.91
  15. Broadbent, D. E., & Broadbent, M. H. (1987). From detection to identification: Response to mul- tiple targets in rapid serial visual presentation. Perception & Psychophysics, 42(2), 105-113. DOI: 10.3758/BF03210498
  16. Carrasco, M., Ling, S., & Read, S. (2004). Attention alters appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 7(3), 308-313. DOI: 10.1038/nn1194
  17. Chalmers, D. J. (1995). Facing up to the problem of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2(3), 200-219.
  18. Chalmers, D. J. (1996). The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  19. Cohen, M. A., Cavanagh, P., Chun, M. M., & Nakayama, K. (2012). The attentional requirements of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(8), 411-417. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.013
  20. Cohen, M. A., & Dennett, D. C. (2011). Consciousness cannot be separated from function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(8), 358-364. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.06.008
  21. Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1990). Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Seminars in the Neurosciences, 2, 263-275. DOI: 10.1016/B978-0-12-185254-2.50021-8
  22. Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1995). Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex? Nature, 375(6527), 121-123. DOI: 10.1038/375121a0
  23. Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1998). Consciousness and neuroscience. Cerebral Cortex, 8(2), 97-107. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/8.2.97
  24. Crick, F., & Koch, C. (2003). A framework for consciousness. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 119- 126. DOI: 10.1038/nn0203-119
  25. Dehaene, S., Changeux, J.-P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, precon- scious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204-211. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007
  26. Dehaene, S., Kerszberg, M., & Changeux, J.-P. (1998). A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 95(24), 14529-14534. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.95.24.14529
  27. Dehaene, S., & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79(1-2), 1-37. DOI: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00123-2
  28. Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., Bihan, D. L., Mangin, J. F., Poline, J. B., & Rivière, D. (2001). Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nature Neu- roscience, 4(7), 752-758. DOI: 10.1038/89551
  29. Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec'H, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., van de Moortele, P. F., & Le Bihan, D. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395(6702), 597-600. DOI: 10.1038/26967
  30. Driver, J., & Mattingley, J. B. (1998). Parietal neglect and visual awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 1(1), 17-22. DOI: 10.1038/217
  31. Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural darwinism: The theory of neuronal group selection. New York, NY: Basic Books.
  32. Edelman, G. M. (1993). Neural darwinism: Selection and reentrant signaling in higher brain function. Neuron, 10(2), 115-125. DOI: 10.1016/0896-6273(93)90304-A
  33. Edelman, G. M., Gally, J. A., & Baars, B. J. (2011). Biology of consciousness. Frontiers in Psychol- ogy, 2, 4. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00004
  34. Edelman, G. M., & Mountcastle, V. B. (1978). The mindful brain: Cortical organization and the group-selective theory of higher brain function. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  35. Edelman, G. M., & Tononi, G. (2000). A universe of consciousness: How matter becomes imagina- tion. New York, NY: Basic Books.
  36. Fahrenfort, J. J., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2012). A true science of consciousness explains phenom- enology: Comment on Cohen and Dennett. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 138-139. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.004
  37. Fahrenfort, J. J., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2007). Masking disrupts reentrant processing in human visual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 1488-1497. DOI: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1488
  38. Felleman, D., & Van Essen, D. C. (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 1(1), 1-47. DOI: 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1
  39. Ferrera, V. P., Rudolph, K. K., & Maunsell, J. H. (1994). Responses of neurons in the parietal and temporal visual pathways during a motion task. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(10), 6171-6186.
  40. Gaillard, R., Dehaene, S., Adam, C., Clémenceau, S., Hasboun, D., Baulac, M., Cohen, L., & Nacchache, L. (2009). Converging intracranial markers of conscious access. PLoS Biology, 7(3), e61. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000061
  41. Galvin, S. J., Podd, J. V, Drga, V., & Whitmore, J. (2003). Type 2 tasks in the theory of signal detectability: Discrimination between correct and incorrect decisions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(4), 843-876. DOI: 10.3758/BF03196546
  42. Goodale, M. A. (2004). An evolving view of duplex vision: Separate but interacting cortical pathways for perception and action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(2), 203-211. DOI: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.002
  43. Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15(1), 20-25. DOI: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8
  44. Goodale, M. A., Milner, A. D., Jakobson, L. S., & Carey, D. P. (1991). A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them. Nature, 349(6305), 154-156. DOI: 10.1038/349154a0
  45. Greenwald, A. G., Draine, S. C., & Abrams, R. L. (1996). Three cognitive markers of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 273(5282), 1699-1702. DOI: 10.1126/science.273.5282.1699
  46. Grill-Spector, K., & Malach, R. (2004). The human visual cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 649-677. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144220
  47. Hameroff, S. (2006a). The entwined mysteries of anesthesia and consciousness: Is there a com- mon underlying mechanism? Anesthesiology, 105(2), 400-412. DOI: 10.1097/00000542-200608000-00024
  48. Hameroff, S. (2006b). Consciousness, neurobiology and quantum mechanics: The case for a connection. In J. A. Tuszynski (Ed.), The emerging physics of consciousness (pp. 193-253). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. DOI: 10.1007/3-540-36723-3_6
  49. Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., & Mangun, G. R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top- down attentional control. Nature Neuroscience, 3(3), 284-291. DOI: 10.1038/72999
  50. Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(1), 12-18. DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00013-X
  51. Lamme, V. A. F. (2004). Separate neural definitions of visual consciousness and visual attention: A case for phenomenal awareness. Neural Networks, 17(5-6), 861-872. DOI: 10.1016/j.neunet. 2004.02.005
  52. Lamme, V. A. F. (2006). Towards a true neural stance on consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(11), 494-501. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.001
  53. Lamme, V. A. F. (2010). How neuroscience will change our view on consciousness. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(3), 204-220. DOI: 10.1080/17588921003731586
  54. Lamme, V. A. F., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 23(11), 571-579. DOI: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01657-X
  55. Lamme, V. A. F., Supèr, H., & Spekreijse, H. (1998). Feedforward, horizontal, and feedback pro- cessing in the visual cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8(4), 529-535. DOI: 10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80042-1
  56. Lamme, V. A. F., Zipser, K., & Spekreijse, H. (2002). Masking interrupts figure-ground signals in V1. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(7), 1044-1053. DOI: 10.1162/089892902320474490
  57. Landman, R., Spekreijse, H., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness. Vision Research, 43(2), 149-164. DOI: 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00402-9
  58. Levi, D. M. (2008). Crowding -An essential bottleneck for object recognition: A mini-review. Vision Research, 48(5), 635-654. DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.12.009
  59. Levine, J. (1983). Materialism and qualia: The explanatory gap. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly, 64(4), 354-361.
  60. Luck, S. J., & Vogel, E. K. (1997). The capacity of visual working memory for features and con- junctions. Nature, 390(6657), 279-281. DOI: 10.1038/36846
  61. Mack, A. (2003). Inattentional blindness: Looking without seeing. Current Directions in Psycho- logical Science, 12(5), 180-184. DOI: 10.1111/1467-8721.01256
  62. Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  63. Macknik, S. L. (2006). Visual masking approaches to visual awareness. Progress in Brain Research, 155, 177-215. DOI: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)55011-3
  64. Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, D. C. (1991). Detection theory: A user's guide. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
  65. Maniscalco, B., & Lau, H. (2012). A signal detection theoretic approach for estimating meta- cognitive sensitivity from confidence ratings. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 422-430. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.09.021
  66. Marcel, A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15(2), 197-237. DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(83)90009-9
  67. Marchetti, G. (2012). Against the view that consciousness and attention are fully dissociable. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 36. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00036
  68. Martens, S., & Wyble, B. (2010). The attentional blink: Past, present, and future of a blind spot in perceptual awareness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(6), 947-957. DOI: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.005
  69. Maunsell, J. H. R., & Newsome, W. T. (1987). Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 10, 363-401. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.ne.10.030187.002051
  70. McFadden, J. (2002a). Synchronous firing and its influence on the brains electromagnetic field. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9(4), 23-50.
  71. McFadden, J. (2002b). The conscious electromagnetic information (Cemi) field theory: The hard problem made easy? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9(8), 45-60.
  72. Merigan, W. H., & Maunsell, J. H. (1993). How parallel are the primate visual pathways? Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 369-402. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.002101
  73. Merikle, P., Smilek, D., & Eastwood, J. (2001). Perception without awareness: Perspectives from cognitive psychology. Cognition, 79(1-2), 115-134. DOI: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00126-8
  74. Mesulam, M. M. (1998). From sensation to cognition. Brain, 121(6), 1013-1052. DOI: 10.1093/brain/121.6.1013
  75. Miller, S. M. (2001). Binocular rivalry and the cerebral hemispheres: With a note on the cor- relates and constitution of visual consciousness. Brain and Mind, 2(1), 119-149. DOI: 10.1023/A:1017981619014
  76. Miller, S. M. (2007). On the correlation/constitution distinction problem (and other hard prob- lems) in the scientific study of consciousness. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 19(3), 159-176. DOI: 10.1111/j.1601-5215.2007.00207.x
  77. Miller, S. M. (Ed.). (2013). The constitution of visual consciousness: Lessons from binocular rivalry. Advances in Consciousness Research (Vol. 90). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company. DOI: 10.1075/aicr.90
  78. Milner, A., & Goodale, M. A. (1993). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Pro- gress in Brain Research, 95, 317-337. DOI: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60379-9
  79. Mishkin, M., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1982). Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial func- tions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys. Behavioural Brain Research, 6(1), 57-77. DOI: 10.1016/0166-4328(82)90081-X
  80. Moutoussis, K., & Zeki, S. (1997a). A direct demonstration of perceptual asynchrony in vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 264(1380), 393-399. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0056
  81. Moutoussis, K., & Zeki, S. (1997b). Functional segregation and temporal hierarchy of the vis- ual perceptive systems. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 264(1387), 1407-1414. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.1997.0196
  82. Moutoussis, K., & Zeki, S. (2002). The relationship between cortical activation and perception investigated with invisible stimuli. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99(14), 9527-9532. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.142305699
  83. Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review, 83(4), 435-450. DOI: 10.2307/2183914
  84. Nakamura, K., Dehaene, S., Jobert, A., Le Bihan, D., & Kouider, S. (2007). Task-specific change of unconscious neural priming in the cerebral language network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104(49), 19643-19648. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0704487104
  85. Oizumi, M., Albantakis, L., & Tononi, G. (2014). From the phenomenology to the mechanisms of consciousness: Integrated Information Theory 3.0. PLoS Computational Biology, 10(5), e1003588. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003588
  86. O'Regan, J. K., Rensink, R. A., & Clark, J. J. (1999). Change-blindness as a result of "mud- splashes". Nature, 398(6722), 34. DOI: 10.1038/17953
  87. Owen, A. M., & Coleman, M. R. (2008). Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1129, 130-138. DOI: 10.1196/annals.1417.018
  88. Owen, A. M., Coleman, M. R., Boly, M., Davis, M. H., Laureys, S., & Pickard, J. D. (2006). Detect- ing awareness in the vegetative state. Science, 313(5792), 1402. DOI: 10.1126/science.1130197
  89. Pelli, D. G., Palomares, M., & Majaj, N. J. (2004). Crowding is unlike ordinary masking: Distin- guishing feature integration from detection. Journal of Vision, 4(12), 12. DOI: 10.1167/4.12.12
  90. Pelli, D. G., & Tillman, K. A. (2008). The uncrowded window of object recognition. Nature Neu- roscience, 11(10), 1129-1135. DOI: 10.1038/nn.2187
  91. Persaud, N., McLeod, P., & Cowey, A. (2007). Post-decision wagering objectively measures awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 10(2), 257-261. DOI: 10.1038/nn1840
  92. Pinto, Y., Sligte, I. G., Shapiro, K. L., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2013). Fragile visual short-term mem- ory is an object-based and location-specific store. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(4), 732-739. DOI: 10.3758/s13423-013-0393-4
  93. Pöppel, E., Brinkmann, R., Von Cramon, D., & Singer, W. (1978). Association and dissociation of visual functions in a case of bilateral occipital lobe infarction. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 225(1), 1-21. DOI: 10.1007/BF00367348
  94. Pöppel, E., Held, R., & Frost, D. (1973). Residual visual function after brain wounds involving the central visual pathways in man. Nature, 243(5405), 295-296. DOI: 10.1038/243295a0
  95. Posner, M. I. (1994). Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 91(16), 7398-7403. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.91.16.7398
  96. Posner, M. I. (2012). Attentional networks and consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 64. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00064
  97. Potter, M. C., Chun, M. M., Banks, B. S., & Muckenhoupt, M. (1998). Two attentional deficits in serial target search: The visual attentional blink and an amodal task-switch deficit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(4), 979-992. DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.24.4.979
  98. Price, N. S. C. (2013). Overview of visual system structure and function. In S. M. Miller (Ed.), The constitution of visual consciousness: Lessons from binocular rivalry (pp. 37-76). Advances in Consciousness Research (Vol. 90). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publish- ing Company. DOI: 10.1075/aicr.90.03pri
  99. Raffone, A., & Pantani, M. (2010). A global workspace model for phenomenal and access con- sciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(2), 580-596. DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.013
  100. Raymond, J. E., Shapiro, K. L., & Arnell, K. M. (1992). Temporary suppression of visual process- ing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 849-860. DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.849
  101. Rees, G., Kreiman, G., & Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(4), 261-270. DOI: 10.1038/nrn783
  102. Rees, G., Wojciulik, E., Clarke, K., Husain, M., Frith, C., & Driver, J. (2000). Unconscious activa- tion of visual cortex in the damaged right hemisphere of a parietal patient with extinction. Brain, 123(8), 1624-1633. DOI: 10.1093/brain/123.8.1624
  103. Rees, G., Wojciulik, E., Clarke, K., Husain, M., Frith, C., & Driver, J. (2002). Neural correlates of conscious and unconscious vision in parietal extinction. Neurocase, 8(5), 387-393. DOI: 10.1093/neucas/8.5.387
  104. Rensink, R. A., O'Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8(5), 368-373. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00427.x
  105. Revonsuo, A. (2001). Can functional brain imaging discover consciousness in the brain? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(3), 3-23.
  106. Roe, A. W., Chelazzi, L., Connor, C. E., Conway, B. R., Fujita, I., Gallant, J. L., Lu, H., & Vanduffel, W. (2012). Toward a unified theory of visual area V4. Neuron, 74(1), 12-29. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.011
  107. Roelfsema, P. R. (2006). Cortical algorithms for perceptual grouping. Annual Review of Neurosci- ence, 29, 203-227. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112939
  108. Seidemann, E., Poirson, A. B., Wandell, B. A., & Newsome, W. T. (1999). Color signals in area MT of the macaque monkey. Neuron, 24(4), 911-917. DOI: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81038-7
  109. Seth, A. K. (2009). Explanatory correlates of consciousness: Theoretical and computational chal- lenges. Cognitive Computation, 1(1), 50-63. DOI: 10.1007/s12559-009-9007-x
  110. Seth, A. K. (2010). The grand challenge of consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 5. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00005
  111. Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28(9), 1059-1074. DOI: 10.1068/p2952
  112. Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (1997). Change blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(7), 261- 267. DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01080-2
  113. Simons, D. J., & Rensink, R. A. (2005). Change blindness: Past, present, and future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(1), 16-20. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.006
  114. Sligte, I. G., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2008). Are there multiple visual short-term mem- ory stores? PLoS ONE, 3(2), e1699. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001699
  115. Sligte, I. G., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2009). V4 activity predicts the strength of visual short-term memory representations. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(23), 7432-7438. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0784-09.2009
  116. Sligte, I. G., Wokke, M. E., Tesselaar, J. P., Scholte, H. S., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2010). Magnetic stim- ulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dissociates fragile visual short-term memory from visual working memory. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1578-1588. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.010
  117. Sperling, G. (1960). Negative afterimage without prior positive image. Science, 131(3413), 1613- 1614. DOI: 10.1126/science.131.3413.1613
  118. Stoerig, P., & Cowey, A. (2007). Blindsight. Current Biology, 17(19), R822-R824. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.016
  119. Tononi, G. (2004). An information integration theory of consciousness. BMC Neuroscience, 5, 42. DOI: 10.1186/1471-2202-5-42
  120. Tononi, G. (2008). Consciousness as integrated information: A provisional manifesto. Biological Bulletin, 215(3), 216-242. DOI: 10.2307/25470707
  121. Tononi, G., & Edelman, G. M. (1998). Consciousness and complexity. Science, 282(5395), 1846- 1851. DOI: 10.1126/science.282.5395.1846
  122. Tononi, G., & Koch, C. (2008). The neural correlates of consciousness: An update. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 239-261. DOI: 10.1196/annals.1440.004
  123. Tononi, G., & Sporns, O. (2003). Measuring information integration. BMC Neuroscience, 4, 31. DOI: 10.1186/1471-2202-4-31
  124. Tovee, M. J. (1994). How fast is the speed of thought? Current Biology, 4(12), 1125-1127. DOI: 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00253-0
  125. Treue, S. (2003). Visual attention: The where, what, how and why of saliency. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13(4), 428-432. DOI: 10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00105-3
  126. Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8(8), 1096-1101. DOI: 10.1038/nn1500
  127. van Boxtel, J. J. A., Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2010a). Consciousness and attention: On suffi- ciency and necessity. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 217. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00217
  128. van Boxtel, J. J. A., Tsuchiya, N., & Koch, C. (2010b). Opposing effects of attention and con- sciousness on afterimages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107(19), 8883-8888. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0913292107
  129. Vandenbroucke, A. R., Sligte, I. G., & Lamme, V. A. F. (2011). Manipulations of attention dis- sociate fragile visual short-term memory from visual working memory. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1559-1568. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.044
  130. Wallis, T. S. A., & Bex, P. J. (2011). Visual crowding is correlated with awareness. Current Biology, 21(3), 254-258. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.01.011
  131. Watanabe, M., Cheng, K., Murayama, Y., Ueno, K., Asamizuya, T., Tanaka, K., & Logothetis, N. (2011). Attention but not awareness modulates the BOLD signal in the human V1 during binocular suppression. Science, 334(6057), 829-831. DOI: 10.1126/science.1203161
  132. Weiskrantz, L., Barbur, J. L., & Sahraie, A. (1995). Parameters affecting conscious versus uncon- scious visual discrimination with damage to the visual cortex (V1). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 92(13), 6122-6126. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.92.13.6122
  133. Weiskrantz, L., Warrington, E. K., Sanders, M. D., & Marshall, J. (1974). Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following a restricted occipital ablation. Brain, 97(4), 709-728. DOI: 10.1093/brain/97.1.709
  134. Whitney, D., & Levi, D. M. (2011). Visual crowding: A fundamental limit on conscious percep- tion and object recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(4), 160-168. DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.02.005
  135. Wilke, M., Logothetis, N. K., & Leopold, D. A. (2003). Generalized flash suppression of salient visual targets. Neuron, 39(6), 1043-1052. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2003.08.003
  136. Wolfe, J. M. (1984). Reversing ocular dominance and suppression in a single flash. Vision Research, 24(5), 471-478. DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(84)90044-0
  137. Zeki, S. (2001). Localization and globalization in conscious vision. Annual Review of Neurosci- ence, 24, 57-86. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.57
  138. Zeki, S. (2008). The disunity of consciousness. Progress in Brain Research, 168, 11-18. DOI: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)68002-9
  139. Zeki, S., & Bartels, A. (1998a). The autonomy of the visual systems and the modularity of conscious vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 353(1377), 1911-1914. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0343
  140. Zeki, S., & Bartels, A. (1998b). The asynchrony of consciousness. Proceedings of the Royal Soci- ety of London B: Biological Sciences, 265(1405), 1583-1585. DOI: 10.1098/rspb.1998.0475
  141. Zeki, S., & ffytche, D. H. (1998). The Riddoch syndrome: Insights into the neurobiology of con- scious vision. Brain, 121(1), 25-45. DOI: 10.1093/brain/121.1.25
  142. Zipser, K., Lamme, V. A. F., & Schiller, P. H. (1996). Contextual modulation in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 16(22), 7376-7389.