Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Outline

Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space

1997

Abstract

Studies of saccadic suppression and induced motion have suggested separate representations of visual space for perception and visually guided behavior. Because these methods required stimulus motion, subjects might have confounded motion and position. We separated cognitive and sensorimotor maps without motion of target, background, or eye, with an "induced Roelofs effect": a target inside an off-center frame appears biased opposite the direction of the frame. A frame displayed to the left of a subject's center line, for example, will make a target inside the frame appear farther to the right than its actual position. The effect always influences perception, but in half of our subjects it did not influence pointing. Cognitive and sensorimotor maps interacted when the motor response was delayed; all subjects now showed a Roelofs effect for pointing, suggesting that the motor system was being fed from the biased cognitive map. A second experiment showed similar results when subjects made an open-ended cognitive response instead of a five-alternative forced choice. Experiment 3 showed that the results were not due to shifts in subjects' perception of the felt straight-ahead position. In Experiment 4, subjects pointed to the target and judged its location on the same trial. Both measures showed a Roelofs effect, indicating that each trial was treated as a single event and that the cognitive representation was accessed to localize this event in both response modes.

References (39)

  1. ABRAMS, R. A., & LANDGRAF, J. Z. (1990). Differential use of distance and location information for spatial localization. Perception & Psy- chophysics, 47, 349-359.
  2. BACON, J. H., GORDON, A., & SCHULMAN, P. H. (1982). The effect of two types of induced-motion displays on perceived location of the in- duced target. Perception & Psychophysics, 32, 353-359.
  3. BRENNER, E., & SMEETS, J. 8. J. (1994). Different frames of reference for position and motion. Naturwissenschaften, 81,30-32.
  4. BRIDGEMAN, B. (1986). Multiple sources of outflow in processing spa- tial information. Acta Psychologica, 63, 35-48.
  5. BRIDGEMAN, B., HENDRY, D., & STARK, L. (1975). Failure to detect dis- placement of the visual world during saccadic eye movements. Vi- sion Research, 15,719-722.
  6. BRIDGEMAN, B., KIRCH, M., & SPERLING, A. (1981). Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion. Perception & Psychophysics, 29, 336-342.
  7. BRIDGEMAN, B., & KLASSEN, H. (1983). On the origin of stroboscopic induced motion. Perception & Psychophysics, 34, 149-154.
  8. BRIDGEMAN, 8., LEWIS, S., HEIT, G., & NAGLE, M. (1979). Relation be- tween cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position per- ception. Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance.S, 692-700.
  9. BRIDGEMAN, 8., & STAGGS, D. (1982). Plasticity in human blindsight. Vision Research, 22,1199-1203.
  10. BRIDGEMAN, 8., & STARK, L. (1979). Omnidirectional increase in thresh- old for image shifts during saccadic eye movements. Perception & Psychophysics, 25, 241-243.
  11. BROSGOLE, L. (1968). An analysis of induced motion. Acta Psycholog- ica, 28, 1-44.
  12. BRUNE, E, & LOCKING, C. (1969). Oculomotorik, Bewegungswahr- nehmung und Raumkonstanz der Sehdinge [The oculomotor system, motion perception and space constancyof visual objects]. Der Nerven- arzt, 40, 692-700.
  13. DASSONVILLE, P., SCHLAG, J., & SCHLAG-REY, M. (1992). Oculomotor localization relies on a damped representation of saccadic eye dis- placement in human and nonhuman primates. Visual Neuroscience, 9,261-269.
  14. DITCHBURN, R. (1955). Eye-movements in relation to retinal action. Op- ticaActa,I,171-176.
  15. FELLEMAN, D. 1., & VAN ESSEN, D. C. (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 1, 1-47.
  16. FESTINGER, L., & CANON, L. K. (1965). Information about spatialloca- tion based on knowledge about efference. Psychological Review, 72, 373-384.
  17. FOLEY, J. M., & HELD, R. (1972). Visuallydirected pointing as a function of target distance, direction, and available cues. Perception & Psy- chophysics, 12,263-268.
  18. GOODALE, M. A., MEENAN, J. P, BULTHOFF, H. H., NICOLLE, D. A., MURPHY, K. J., & RACICOT, CI. (1994). Separate neural pathways for the visual analysis of object shape in perception and prehension. Cur- rent Biology, 4, 604-610.
  19. GOODALE, M. A., PELISSON, D., & PRABLANC, C. (1986). Large adjust- ments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement. Nature, 320, 748-750.
  20. HALLETT, P. E., & LIGHTSTONE, A. D. (1976). Saccadic eye movements towards stimuli triggered during prior saccades. Vision Research, 16, 99-106.
  21. HANSEN, R. (1979). Spatial localization during pursuit eye movements. Vision Research, 16, 1213-1221.
  22. HANSEN, R., & SKAVENSKI, A. (1977). Accuracy of eye-position infor- mation for motor control. Vision Research, 17, 919-926.
  23. HONDA, H. (1985). Spatial localization in saccade and pursuit-eye- movement conditions: A comparison of perceptual and motor mea- sures. Perception & Psychophysics, 38, 41-46.
  24. HONDA, H. ( 1990).The extraretinal signal from the pursuit-eye-movement system: Its role in the perceptual and the egocentric localization sys- tems. Perception & Psychophysics, 48, 509-515.
  25. MACK, A. (1970). An investigation of the relationship between eye and retinal image movement in the perception of movement. Perception & Psychophysics, 8, 291-298.
  26. MASSON, G., PROTEAU, L., & MESTRE, D. R. (1995). Effects of station- ary and moving textured backgrounds on the visuo-oculo-rnanual tracking in humans. Vision Research, 35,837-852.
  27. MATEEFF, S., & GOUREVICH, A. (1983). Peripheral vision and perceived visual direction. Biological Cybernetics, 49, 111-118.
  28. PAILLARD, J. (1987). Cognitive versus sensorimotor encoding of spatial information. In P Ellen & C. Thinus-Blanc (Eds.), Cognitive pro- cesses and spatial orientation in animal and man (Vol. 2, pp. 43-77). Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff.
  29. PAILLARD, J. (1991). Motor and representational framing of space. In 1. Paillard (Ed.), Brain and space (pp. 163-182). Oxford: Oxford Uni- versity Press.
  30. POST, R. 8., & LEIBOWITZ, H. W. (1985). A revised analysis of the role of efference in motion perception. Perception, 14,631-643.
  31. PRABLANC, C, EcHALLIER, J. E, KOMILIS, E., & JEANNEROD, M. (1979). Optimal response of eye and hand motor systems in pointing. Bio- logical Cybernetics, 45,113-124.
  32. ROEDIGER, H. L., III, WELDON, M., & CHALLIS, 8. (1988). Explaining dissociations between implicit and explicit measures of retention: A processing account. In H. L. Roediger 111 & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Va- rieties ofmemory and consciousness: Essays in honour ofEndel Tul- ving(pp. 3-41). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  33. ROELOFS, C. (1935). Optische Localisation [Optical localization]. Archiv fiir Augenheilkunde, 109,395-415.
  34. SMEETS, J. 8. J., & BRENNER, E. (1995). Perception and action are based on the same visual information: Distinction between position and ve- locity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 21, )9-31.
  35. SUGARMAN, R., & COHEN, W. (1968). Perceived target displacement as a function offield movement and asymmetry. Perception & Psycho- physics, 3, 169-173.
  36. WALLACH, H., & LEWIS, C (1965). The effect of abnormal displace- ment of the retinal image during eye movements. Perception & Psy- chophysics, 1,25-29.
  37. WEISKRANTZ, L., WARRINGTON, D., SANDERS, M., & MARSHALL, J. (1974). Visual capacity in thehemianopic field following restricted occipital ablation. Brain, 97, 709-729.
  38. WERNER, H., WAPNER, S., & BRUELL, J. (1953). Experiments on sensory- tonic field theory of perception: VI. The effect of position of head, eyes, and ofobject on the position of the apparent median plane. Jour- nal ofExperimental Psychology, 46,293-299.
  39. WONG, E., & MACK. A. (1981). Saccadic programming and perceived location. Acta Psychologica, 48, 123-131. (Manuscript received June 2, 1995; revision accepted for publication May 20, 1996.)