Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Outline

Cultural learning and creation

Abstract

Human children become cultural beings by learning to participate in the cultural activities and practices going on around them. Household pets grow up in the midst of these same cultural activities and practices, but they do not learn to participate in them in anything like the same way as human children. Even chimpanzees and bonobos raised in human homes and treated like human children still retain, for the most part, their species-typical social and cognitive skills without turning into cultural beings of the human kind. This difference suggests that humans are biologically adapted, in ways that other animal species are not, for becoming cultural beings by tuning in to what others around them are doing, and thereby learning from them. Moreover, on occasion, young children even create with others smallscale cultural activities and routines involving one or another form of collaboration, or even collaborative pretense. Such cultural creation would also seem to be unique to human beings, and of course cultural creation leads to ever new cultural environments in which human cognitive ontogeny takes place.

References (141)

  1. Akhtar, N. (2005). The robustness of learning through overhearing. Developmental Science, 8(2), 199-209.
  2. Aldridge, M., Stone, K., Sweeney, M. & Bower, T. (2000) Preverbal children with autism understand the intentions of others. Developmental Science 3:294-301.
  3. Alexander, R. D. (1987). The biology of moral systems. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter. American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM-IV). American Psychiatric Association.
  4. Baldwin, D. A. (1993). Infants' ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference. Journal of Child Language, 20(2), 395-418.
  5. Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7:113-128.
  6. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, USA: MIT Press.
  7. Barresi, J., & Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19(1), 107-154.
  8. Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205-224.
  9. Behne, T., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. (2005). One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game. Developmental Science 8:6 (2005), pp 492-499
  10. Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Unwilling versus unable: Infants' understanding of intentional action. Developmental Psychology, 41, 328-337.
  11. Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A., & Gattis, M. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 153-164.
  12. Bellagamba, F., & Tomasello, M. (1999). Re-enacting intended acts: Comparing 12-and 18- month-olds. Infant Behavior & Development, 22, 277-282.
  13. Bigelow, A. E., MacLean, K., & Proctor, J. (2004). The role of joint attention in the development of infants' play with objects. Developmental Science, 7(5), 518-526.
  14. Bråten, S. (2000) Modellmakt og altersentriske spedbarn. Sigma.
  15. Bratman, M. E. (1992). Shared cooperative activity. Philosophical Review, 101(2), 327-341.
  16. Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). The importance of eyes: How infants interpret adult looking behavior. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 958-966.
  17. Bruner, J. (1983). Child's talk. New York: Norton.
  18. Butler, S. C., Caron, A. J., & Brooks, R. (2000). Infant understanding of the referential nature of looking. Journal of Cognition and Development, 4, 359-377.
  19. Butterworth, G., & Jarrett, N. (1991). What Minds Have in Common Is Space -Spatial Mechanisms Serving Joint Visual-Attention in Infancy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 55-72.
  20. Butterworth, G., 1995. Origins of mind in perception and action. In: Moore, C., Dunham, P.J. (Eds.), Joint Attention: Its Origins and Role in Development. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 29-40.
  21. Call, J., & Carpenter, M. (2003). On imitation in apes and children. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 26, 325-349.
  22. Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Distinguishing intentional from accidental actions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112(2), 192-206.
  23. Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). What do chimpanzees know about seeing revisited: an explanation of the third kind. In N. Eilan, C. Hoerl, T. McCormack, & J. Roessler (Eds.), Issues in joint attention (pp. 45-64). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  24. Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Cues the chimpanzees do and do not use to find hidden objects. Animal Cognition, 3(1), 23-34.
  25. Call, J., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Copying results and copying actions in the process of social learning: Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 151-163.
  26. Call, J., Hare, B. A., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Chimpanzee gaze following in an object- choice task. Animal Cognition, 1(2), 89-99.
  27. Call, J., Hare, B., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2004). 'Unwilling' versus 'unable': Chimpanzees' understanding of human intentions. Developmental Science, 7, 488-498.
  28. Caron, A. J., Butler, S. C., & Brooks, R. (2002). Gaze following at 12 and 14 months: Do the eyes matter? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 225-239.
  29. Caron, A. J., Kiel, E. J., Dayton, M., & Butler, S. C. (2002). Comprehension of the referential intent of looking and pointing between 12 and 15 months. Journal of Cognition & Development, 3(4), 445-464.
  30. Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen-to 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315-330.
  31. Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2002). Understanding 'prior intentions' enables 2- year-olds to imitatively learn a complex task. Child Development, 73, 1431-1441.
  32. Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Twelve-and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals. Developmental Science, 8, F13-F20.
  33. Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998b). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), 176.
  34. Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2001). Understanding of others' intentions in children with autism and children with developmental delays. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 589-599.
  35. Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33(5), 781-789.
  36. Corkum, V., & Moore, C. (1995). Development of joint visual attention in infants. In Moore, Chris (Ed);
  37. Dunham, Philip J. (Ed). (1995). Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. (pp. 61-83). vii, 286pp.
  38. Corkum, V., & Moore, C. (1998). The origins of joint visual attention in infants. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 28-38.
  39. Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological and referential understanding of action in infancy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 447-458.
  40. Deak, G. O., Flom, R. A., & Pick, A. D. (2000). Effects of gesture and target on 12-and 18- month-olds' joint visual attention to objects in front of or behind them. Developmental Psychology, 36(4), 511-523.
  41. Desrochers, S., Morissette, P., & Ricard, M. (1995). Two perspectives on pointing in infancy. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 85-101). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  42. Dunham, P. J., Dunham, F., & Curwin, A. (1993). Joint-Attentional States and Lexical Acquisition at 18 Months. Developmental Psychology, 29(5), 827-831.
  43. Dunphy-Lelii, S., & Wellman, H. M. (2004). Infants' understanding of occlusion of others' line-of-sight: Implications for an emerging theory of mind. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1(1), 49-66.
  44. Eilan, N. 2005. Joint attention, communication, and mind. In Eilan, N., Hoerl, C., McCormack, T., and Roessler, J. (eds.). Joint Attention: Communication and Other Minds Issues in Philosophy and Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  45. Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, 5th ed.: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (Vol. 3, pp. 701-778). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
  46. El`Konin, D. B. (1966). Symbolics and its function in the play of children. Soviet Education, 8, 35-41.
  47. Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425, 785-791.
  48. Fernyhough, C. (1996) The dialogic mind: A dialogic approach to the higher mental functions. New Ideas in Psychology 14:47-62.
  49. Flavell, J. H. (1992). Perspectives on perspective-taking. In H. Beilin & P. B. Pufall (Eds.), Piaget's theory: Prospects and possibilities. The Jean Piaget symposium series (Vol. 14, pp. 107-139).
  50. Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., & Green, F. L. (1987). Young children's knowledge about the apparent-real and the pretend-real distinctions. Developmental Psychology, 23(5), 816- 822.
  51. Flavell, J. H., Shipstead, S. G., & Croft, K. (1978). Young children's knowledge about visual perception: Hiding objects from others. Child Development, 49, 1208-1211.
  52. Floor P., & Akhtar, N. (2006). Can 18-month-old infants learn words by listening in on conversations? Infancy, 9, 327-339.
  53. Franco, F., & Butterworth, G. (1996). Pointing and social awareness: Declaring and requesting in the second year. Journal of Child Language, 23(2), 307-336.
  54. Gergely, G. (2003) What should a robot learn from an infant? Mechanisms of action interpretation and observational learning in infancy. In Prince, C. G., Berthouze, L., Kozima, H., Bullock, D., Stojanov, G. & Balkenius, C.(Eds). Proceedings Third International Workshop on Epigenetic Robotics: Modeling Cognitive Development in Robotic Systems 101, pages pp. 13-24, Boston, MA, USA.
  55. Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (in press). Sylvia's recipe: Human culture, imitation, and pedagogy. In N. Enfield & S. Levinson (Eds.), The roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition, and interaction. Oxford: Berg.
  56. Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infance: the naive theory of rational action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(7), 287-292.
  57. Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.
  58. Gomez, J. C., & Martin-Andrade, B. (2005). Fantasy play in animals. In A. Pellegrini & P. K. Smith (Eds.), The nature of play: Great apes and humans (pp. 139-171). New York: Guilford.
  59. Gräfenhain, M., Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (submitted). One-year-olds' understanding of nonverbally expressed communicative intentions directed to a third person.
  60. Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour, 61(1), 139-151.
  61. Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (in press). Chimpanzees deceive a human by hiding. Cognition.
  62. Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behaviour, 59, 771-785.
  63. Heal, J.(2005). Joint attention and understanding the mind. In Eilan, N., Hoerl, C., McCormack, T., and Roessler, J. (eds.). Joint Attention: Communication and Other Minds Issues in Philosophy and Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  64. Hobson, P. (1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  65. Hobson, R. P.(2002) The cradle of thought. Macmillan.
  66. Husserl, E. (2004). Wahrnehmung und Aufmerksamkeit. Texte aus dem Nachlass (1893- 1912) (T. Vongehr & R. Giuliani, Eds.), Vol. 38, Husserliana. Hamburg: Springer.
  67. Itakura, S. (1996). An exploratory study of gaze monitoring in nonhuman primates.
  68. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt. Japanese Psychological Research, 38, 174-180.
  69. Jensen, K., Hare, B., Call, J., Tomasello, M. (2006). Are chimpanzees spiteful or altruistic when sharing food? Proceedings of the Royal Society: B. 273, 1013-1021.
  70. Johnson, S. C., Booth, A., & O'Hearn, K. (2001). Inferring the goals of a nonhuman agent. Cognitive Development 16, 637 -656.
  71. Kaye K. (1982). The mental and social life of babies: How parents create persons. University of Chicago Press.
  72. Leekam, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Perrett, D., Milders, M., & Brown, S. (1997). Eye-direction detection: A dissociation between geometric and joint attention skills in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 77-95.
  73. Leslie, A. M. (1984) Spatiotemporal continuity and the perception of causality in infants. Perception 13:287-305.
  74. Liebal, K. Colombi, C., Rogers, S., Warneken, F., and Tomasello, M (submitted). Cooperative Activities in Children with Autism.
  75. Lillard, A. S. (1993). Young children's conceptualization of pretense: Action or mental representational state? Child Development, 64(2), 372-386.
  76. Lillard, A. S. (1998). Wanting to be it: Children's understanding of intentions underlying pretense. Child Development, 69(4), 981-993.
  77. Liszkowski, U. (2005). Human twelve-month-olds point cooperatively to share interest with and provide information for a communicative partner. Gesture, 5 (1,2). 135-154.
  78. Liszkowski, U. (2006). Infant Pointing at Twelve Months: Communicative Goals, Motives, and Social-Cognitive Abilities: To appear in: Enfield, N. & Levinson, S. (Eds.) The roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition, and interaction. Oxford: Berg.
  79. Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (submitted) Reference and attitude in infant pointing.
  80. Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Henning, A., Striano, T. & Tomasello, M. (2004). Twelve- month-olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science 7:297-307.
  81. Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Twelve-and 18-month- olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition and Development 7(2).
  82. Lord, C. (1984) The development of peer relations in children with autism. In: Advances in applied developmental psychology, ed. F. Morrison, C. Lord, & D. Keating. Academic.
  83. Loveland, K. A., McEvoy, R. E., & Tunali, B. (1990). Narrative story telling in autism and Down's syndrome. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8(1), 9-23.
  84. Masangkay, Z. S., McCluskey, K., McIntyre, C. W., Sims-Knight, J., Vaughn, B. E., & Flavell, J. H. (1974). The early development of inferences about the visual percepts of others. Child Development, 45, 357-366.
  85. McGuigan, N., & Doherty, M. J. (2002). The relation between hiding skill and judgment of eye direction in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 38(3), 418-427.
  86. Melis, A. P., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (in press). Chimpanzees conceal visual and auditory information from others. Journal of Comparative Psychology.
  87. Melis, A. P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Chimpanzees Recruit the Best Collaborators. Science, 311(5765), 1297-1300.
  88. Meltzoff, A. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 1-16.
  89. Meltzoff, A. N., & Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism (pp. 335-366). New York: Oxford.
  90. Michotte, A. (1963). The perception of causality. London: Methuen & Co.
  91. Moll, H. & Tomasello, M. (2004). 12-and 18-month-olds follow gaze behind barriers. Developmental Science, 7, F1-F9.
  92. Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Twelve-and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barriers. Developmental Science, 7(1), F1-F9.
  93. Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (in press-a). Level 1 perspective-taking at 24 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology.
  94. Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (in press-b). How 14-and 18-month-olds know what's new for others. Developmental Psychology.
  95. Moll, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (under revision). Fourteen-month-old infants know what others know only from joint engagement.
  96. Moll, H., Koring, C., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (in press). Infants determine others' focus of attention by pragmatics and exclusion. Journal of Cognition and Development.
  97. Moore, C., & Corkum, V. (1994). Social Understanding at the End of the 1st Year of Life. Developmental Review, 14(4), 349-372.
  98. Moore, C., & D'Entremont, B. (2001). Developmental changes in pointing as a function of attentional focus. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2 (2), 109-129.
  99. Myowa, M. (1996) Imitation of facial gestures by an infant chimpanzee. Primates 37:207-13.
  100. Myowa-Yamakoshi M. & Matsuzawa T. (2000).Imitation of intentional manipulatory actions in chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114, 381-391.
  101. Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., Tomonaga, M., Tanaka, M. & Matsuzawa, T. (2004) Imitation in neonatal chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Developmental Science 7:437-42.
  102. Nichols, S., & Stich, S. (2000). A cognitive theory of pretense. Cognition, 74, 115-147.
  103. Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-Month-Old Infants Understand False Beliefs? Science, 308 (5719), 255-258.
  104. Perner, J., Baker, S., & Hutton, D. (1994). Prelief: The conceptual origins of belief and pretence. In C. Lewis & P. Mitchell (Eds.), Children's early understanding of mind: Origins and development. (pp. 261-286). Hove, England UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  105. Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.
  106. Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1996). What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61(3).
  107. Povinelli, D.J., Reaux, J.E., Bierschwale, D.T., Allain, A.D., & Simon, B.B. (1997). Exploitation of pointing as a referential gesture in young children, but not adolescent chimpanzees.Cognitive Development, 12 (4), 327-365.
  108. Rakoczy, H. (in press). Pretend play and the development of collective intentionality. Cognitive Systems Research (special issue on "Cognition and Collective Intentionality".
  109. Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (in press). Two-year-olds' grasp the intentional structure of pretense acts. Developmental Science.
  110. Rakoczy, H., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2004). Young children know that trying is not pretending -a test of the "behaving-as-if" contrual of children's early concept of "pretense". Developmental Psychology, 40(3), 388-399.
  111. Rakoczy, H., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2005). On tools and toys: How children learn to act on and pretend with 'virigin' objects. Developmental Science, 8(1), 57-73.
  112. Rakoczy, H., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (in press). The role of experience and discourse in children's develping understanding of pretend play actions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology.
  113. Ross, H. S., & Lollis, S. P. (1987). Communication within infant social games. Developmental Psychology, 23(2), 241-248.
  114. Sabbagh, M. (1999) Communicative intentions and language: Evidence from right- hemisphere damage and autism. Brain and Language, 70: 29-69.
  115. Schwier, C., van Maanen, C., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (in press). Rational imitation in 12-month-old infants.
  116. Searle, J. R. (1995). The construction of social reality. New York: Free Press.
  117. Silk, J.B., Brosnan, S.F., Vonk, J., Henrich, J., Povinelli, D.J., Richardson, A.S., Lambeth, S.P., Mascaro, J., Schapiro, S.J. (2005) Chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelated group members. Nature, 437:1357-1359
  118. Sommerville, J. A. & Woodward, A. L. (2005) Pulling out the intentional structure of action: The relation between action processing and action production in infancy. Cognition 95(1):1-30.
  119. Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  120. Stern, D. N. (1985) The interpersonal world of the infant. Basic.
  121. Tomasello, M. & Farrar, M. J. (1986) Joint Attention and Early Language. Child Development, 57, 1454-1463.
  122. Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA, US: Harvard University Press.
  123. Tomasello, M. (1999b). The cultural ecology of young children's interactions with objects and artifacts. In E. Winograd, R. Fivush, & W. Hirst (Eds.), Ecological Approaches to Cognition: Essays in Honor of Ulric Neisser. Erlbaum.
  124. Tomasello, M. (in press). Why don't apes point? In N. Enfield & S. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of Human Sociality. Wenner-Grenn.
  125. Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2005). The emergence of social cognition in three young chimpanzees. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 70 (1, Serial No. 279).
  126. Tomasello, M., & Haberl, K. (2003). Understanding attention: 12-and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 906-912.
  127. Tomasello, M., Call, J. & Hare, B. (1998) Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspecifics. Animal Behaviour 55:1063-69.
  128. Tomasello, M., Call, J., & Gluckman, A. (1997). Comprehension of novel communicative signs by apes and human children. Child Development, 68 (6), 1067-1080.
  129. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The ontogeny and phylogeny of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675-735.
  130. Tomasello, M., Hare, B., & Agnetta, B. (1998). Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, follow gaze direction geometrically. Animal Behaviour, 58, 769-777.
  131. Tomasello, M., Hare, B., & Fogleman, T. (2001). The ontogeny of gaze-following in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, and rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta. Animal Behaviour, 61, 335-343.
  132. Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural Learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495-552.
  133. Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy. A description of primary intersubjectivity. In: M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before Speech: The Beginning of Human Communication (pp. 321-47), London: Cambridge University Press.
  134. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvad University Press.
  135. Walton, K. L. (1990). Mimesis as make-believe. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  136. Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic Helping in Human Infants and Young Chimpanzees. Science, 311(5765), 1301-1303.
  137. Warneken, F., Chen, F., & Tomasello, M. (in press). Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. Child Development.
  138. Woodward, A. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition, 69, 1-34.
  139. Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition, 69(1), 1-34.
  140. Wyman, E., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (in preparation). Young children's understanding of the context-speficity of pretense.
  141. Zentall, T. (1996) An analysis of imitative learning in animals. In: Social learning in animals: The roots of culture, ed. C. M. Heyes & B. G. Galef Jr. Academic.