Artifact and Tool Categorization
https://doi.org/10.1007/S13164-013-0140-9Abstract
This study addresses the issue of artifact kinds from a psychological and cognitive perspective. The primary interest of the investigation lies in understanding how artifacts are categorized and what are the properties people rely on for their identification. According to a classical philosophical definition artifacts form an autonomous class of instances including all and only those objects that do not exist in nature, but are artificial, in the sense that they are made by an artĭfex. This definition suggests that artifacts are classified primarily on the basis of the recognition of their artificial nature. Nevertheless, many psychological and cognitive studies maintain that artifacts are categorized mainly on the basis of the recognition of the function they have been made to accomplish. Since tools are also categorized primarily on the basis of their function, this would imply that artifacts and tools are represented in the same way. In the study participants categorized a set of objects (denoted by words) once as tools and once as artifacts. Results show that reaction times (RTs) are faster in the artifact categorization condition than in the tool categorization condition. This pattern indicates that artifacts and tools are not represented in the same way and that the identification of the members of each class is carried out in the basis of different criteria.
References (38)
- Bloom, P. 1996. Intention, history, and artifact concepts. Cognition 60: 1-29.
- Bloom, P. 1998. Theories of artifact categorization. Cognition 66: 87-93.
- Bloom, P. 2000. How children learn the meaning of words. Cambridge: MIT Press.
- Brown, A.L. 1990. Domain-specific principles affect learning and transfer in children. Cognitive Science 14: 107- 133.
- Cummins-Sebree, S.E., and D.M. Fragaszy. 2005. Choosing and using tools: Capuchins (Cebus apella) use a different metric than tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Journal of Comparative Psychology 119(2): 210- 219.
- Dennett, D. 1990. The interpretation of texts, people, and other artifacts. Philosophy and Phenomenolog- ical Research 50: 177-194.
- Dellantonio, S., M. Innamorati, and L. Pastore. 2012. Sensing aliveness: an hypothesis on the constitution of the categories 'animate' and 'inanimate'. Integrative Psychological & Behavioral Science 46(2): 172-195.
- Elder, C.L. 2009. On the place of artifacts in ontology. In Creations of the mind. Theories of artifacts and their representation, ed. E. Margolis and S. Laurence, 33-51. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Forster, K.I. 2004. Category size effect revisited: Frequency and masked priming effects in semantic categorization. Brain and Language 90: 276-286.
- Gibson, J.J. 1977. The theory of affordances. In Perceiving, acting, and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology, ed. R. Shaw and J. Bransfor, 127-143. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
- Gould, J.L. 2009. Animal artifacts. In Creations of the mind. Theories of artifacts and their representation, ed. E. Margolis and S. Laurence, 249-266. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Gould, J.L., and C.G. Gould. 1999. The animal mind. New York: W.H. Freeman & Company.
- Grandy, R.E. 2009. Artifacts: Parts and principles. In Creations of the mind. Theories of artifacts and their representation, ed. E. Margolis and S. Laurence, 18-32. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Greif, M.L., D.G.K. Nelson, F.C. Keil, and F. Gutierrez. 2006. What do children want to know about animals and artifacts? Domain-specific requests for information. Psychological Science 17(6): 455- 459.
- Hauser, M.D., and L.R. Santos. 2009. The evolutionary ancestry of our knowledge of tools: From percepts to concepts. In Creations of the mind. Theories of artifacts and their representation, ed. E. Margolis and S. Laurence, 267-288. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Hauser, M.D. 1997. Artifactual kinds and functional and functional design features: What a primate understands without language. Cognition 64(3): 285-308.
- Hespos, S., and R. Baillargeon. 2001. Reasoning about containment events in very young infants. Cognition 78: 207-245.
- Hilpinen, R. 1992. On artifacts and works of art. Theoria 58: 58-82.
- Hilpinen, R. 1993. Authors and artifacts. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society 93: 155-178.
- Hilpinen, R. 2011. Artifact. In The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, ed. E. N. Zalta. URL = <http:// plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2011/entries/artifact/>.
- Keil, F.C. 1989. Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
- Keil, F.C., M.L. Greif, and R.S. Kerner. 2009. A world apart: How concepts of the constructed world are different in representation and in development. In Creations of the mind. Theories of artifacts and their representation, ed. E. Margolis and S. Laurence, 231-245. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Kelemen, D., and S. Carey. 2009. The essence of artifacts: Developing the design stance. In Creations of the mind. Theories of artifacts and their representation, ed. E. Margolis and S. Laurence, 212-230. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Laudanna, A., A.M. Thornton, G. Brown, C. Burani, and L. Marconi. 1995. Un corpus dell'italiano scritto contemporaneo dalla parte del ricevente. In III Giornate internazionali di Analisi Statistica dei Dati Testuali. Volume I, ed. S. Bolasco, L. Lebart, and A. Salem, 103-109. Roma: Cisu.
- Lotto, L., R. Job, and R. Rumiati. 1999. Visual effects in picture and word categorization. Memory & Cognition 27: 674-684.
- Mahon, B.Z., and A. Caramazza. 2009. The organization and representation of conceptual knowledge in the brain: Living kinds and artifacts. In Creations of the mind. Theories of artifacts and their representa- tion, ed. E. Margolis and S. Laurence, 157-187. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Mandler, J.M. 2002. On the foundation of the semantic system. In Category specificity in brain and mind, ed. E.M.E. Forde and G.W. Humphreys, 315-340. New York: Psychology Press.
- Mandler, J.M., and L. McDonough. 1998. Studies in inductive inference in infancy. Cognitive Psychology 37: 60-96.
- McCarrell, N.S., and M.A. Callanan. 1995. Form-function correspondences in children's inference. Child Development 66(2): 532-546.
- Medin, D.L., and A. Ortony. 1989. Psychological essentialism. In Similarity and analogical reasoning, ed. S. Vosniadou and A. Ortony, 179-195. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Mulatti, C., and M. Coltheart. 2012. Picture-word interference and the response-exclusion hypothesis. Cortex 48(3): 363-372.
- Mulatti, C., V. Cembrani, F. Peressotti, and R. Job. 2008. The turple effect is modulated by baseword frequency: Implications for models of lexical and semantic access. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 15(6): 1078-1082.
- Mulatti, C., L. Lotto, F. Peressotti, and R. Job. 2010. Speed of processing explains the picture-word asymmetry in conditional naming. Psychological Research 74: 71-81.
- Santos, L.R., Marc D. Hauser, and E.S. Spelke. 2002. Domain-specific knowledge in human children and non-human primates: Artifact and food kinds. In The cognitive animal: empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition, ed. M. Bekoff, C. Allen, and G. Burghardt, 205-216. USA: MIT Press.
- Searle, J.R. 2009. Social ontology. In Creations of the mind. Theories of artifacts and their representation, ed. E. Margolis and S. Laurence, 3-17. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Sperber, D. 2009. Seedless grapes: Nature and culture. In Creations of the mind. Theories of artifacts and their representation, ed. E. Margolis and S. Laurence, 124-137. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Thomasson, A.L. 2009. Artifacts and human concepts. In Creations of the mind. Theories of artifacts and their representation, ed. E. Margolis and S. Laurence, 52-73. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Vaesen, K. 2012. The cognitive basis of human tool use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35(4): 1-16.