Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Outline

How much of symbolic manipulation is just symbol pushing?

Abstract

This paper explores the hypothesis that schematic abstraction—rule following—is partially implemented through processes and knowledge used to understand motion. Two experiments explore the mechanisms used by reasoners solving simple linear equations with one variable. Participants solved problems displayed against a background that moved rightward or leftward. Solving was facilitated when the background motion moved in the direction of the numeric transposition required to solve for the unknown variable. Previous theorizing has usually assumed that such formal problems are solved through the repeated application of abstract transformation patterns (rules) to equations, replicating the steps produced in typical worked solutions. However, the current results suggest that in addition to such strategies, advanced reasoners often employ a mental motion strategy when manipulating algebraic forms: elements of the problem are “picked up” and “moved” across the equation line. This demonstration supports the suggestion that genuinely schematic reasoning could be implemented in perceptual-motor systems through the simulated transformation of referential (but physical) symbol systems.

References (18)

  1. Anderson, J.R. (2005). Human symbol manipulation within an Integrated Cognitive Architecture. Cognitive Science 29, 313-341
  2. Anderson, J.R. (2007). How can the human mind occur in the physical universe? New York: Oxford University Press.
  3. Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577-660.
  4. Barsalou, L.W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617-645.
  5. Casasanto, D. & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the Mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106, 579-593.
  6. Clark, A. (1998). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  7. Dennett, D.C. (1994). The role of language in intelligence, in Jean Khalfa (ed), What is Intelligence? Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  8. Dorfler, W. (2002). Formation of mathematical objects as decision making. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 4, 337-350.
  9. Endress, A. D., Scholl, B. J., & Mehler, J. (2005). The role of salience in the extraction of algebraic rules. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 406-419.
  10. Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. NY: Crowell.
  11. Landy, D. & Goldstone, R. L. (2007A). Formal notations are diagrams: Evidence from a production task. Memory & Cognition 35(8).
  12. Landy, D. & Goldstone, R. L. (2007B). How abstract is symbolic thought?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 33(4), 720-733.
  13. Marcus, G. F. (2001). The algebraic mind: Integrating connectionism and cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  14. Markman, A. B. & E. Dietrich (2000b). Extending the Classical View of Representation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4(12), 470-475.
  15. Novick, L. R., & Catley, K. M. (2007). Understanding phylogenies in biology: The influence of a Gestalt perceptual principle. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13, 197-223.
  16. Pothos, E. M., Chater, N., & Ziori, E. (2006). Does stimulus appearance affect learning? American Journal of Psychology 119(2) pp. 277-301.
  17. Sloman, S. A. (1996). "The Empirical Case for Two Systems of Reasoning." Psychological Bulletin, 119(1), 3- 22.
  18. Zwaan, R.A., Madden, C.J., Yaxley, R.H., & Aveyard, M.E. (2004). Moving words: Dynamic mental representations in language comprehension. Cognitive Science, 28, 611- 619.