Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Outline

Looking for the self in pathological unconsciousness

2013

https://doi.org/10.3389/FNHUM.2013.00538

Abstract

There is an intimate relationship between consciousness and the notion of self. By studying patients with disorders of consciousness, we are offered with a unique lesion approach to tackle the neural correlates of self in the absence of subjective reports. Studies employing neuroimaging techniques point to the critical involvement of midline anterior and posterior cortices in response to the passive presentation of self-referential stimuli, such as the patient's own name and own face. Also, resting state studies show that these midline regions are severely impaired as a function of the level of consciousness. Theoretical frameworks combining all this progress surpass the functional localization of self-related cognition and suggest a dynamic system-level approach to the phenomenological complexity of subjectivity. Importantly for non-communicating patients suffering from disorders of consciousness, the clinical translation of these technologies will allow medical professionals and families to better comprehend these disorders and plan efficient medical management for these patients.

References (72)

  1. Anderson, J. S., Druzgal, T. J., Lopez- Larson, M., Jeong, E. K., Desai, K., and Yurgelun-Todd, D. (2011). Net- work anticorrelations, global regres- sion, and phase-shifted soft tissue correction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 919-934. doi:10.1002/hbm.21079
  2. Blumenfeld, H. (2012). Impaired con- sciousness in epilepsy. Lancet Neu- rol. 11, 814-826. doi:10.1016/S1474- 4422(12)70188-6
  3. Boly, M., Balteau, E., Schnakers, C., Degueldre, C., Moonen, G., Luxen, A., et al. (2007). Base- line brain activity fluctuations pre- dict somatosensory perception in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
  4. U.S.A. 104, 12187-12192. doi:10. 1073/pnas.0611404104
  5. Boly, M., Faymonville, M.-E., Peigneux, P., Lambermont, B., Damas, P., Del Fiore, G., et al. (2004). Audi- tory processing in severely brain injured patients: differences between the minimally conscious state and the persistent vegetative state. Arch. Neurol. 61, 233-238. doi:10.1001/ archneur.61.2.233
  6. Boly, M., Tshibanda, L., Vanhau- denhuyse, A., Noirhomme, Q., Schnakers, C., Ledoux, D., et al. (2009). Functional connectivity in the default network during resting state is preserved in a vegetative but not in a brain dead patient. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2393-2400. doi:10. 1002/hbm.20672
  7. Boveroux, P., Bonhomme, V., Boly, M., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Maquet, P., and Laureys, S. (2008). Brain func- tion in physiologically, pharmaco- logically, and pathologically altered states of consciousness. Int. Anesthe- siol. Clin. 46, 131-146. doi:10.1097/ AIA.0b013e318181a8b3
  8. Brunetti, M., Della Penna, S., Ferretti, A., Del Gratta, C., Cianflone, F., Belardinelli, P., et al. (2008). A frontoparietal network for spatial attention reorienting in the auditory domain: a human fMRI/MEG study of functional and tempo- ral dynamics. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1139-1147. doi:10.1093/cercor/ bhm145
  9. Bruno, M.-A., Majerus, S., Boly, M., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Schnakers, C., Gosseries, O., et al. (2012). Func- tional neuroanatomy underlying the clinical subcategorization of min- imally conscious state patients. J. Neurol. 259, 1087-1098. doi:10. 1007/s00415-011-6303-7
  10. Bruno, M.-A., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Thibaut, A., Moonen, G., and Lau- reys, S. (2011). From unrespon- sive wakefulness to minimally con- scious PLUS and functional locked- in syndromes: recent advances in our understanding of disor- ders of consciousness. J. Neu- rol. 258, 1373-1384. doi:10.1007/ s00415-011-6114-x
  11. Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 1-38. doi:10.1196/annals.1440.011
  12. Chai, X. J., Castanon, A. N., Ongur, D., and Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. (2012). Anticorrelations in resting state net- works without global signal regres- sion. Neuroimage 59, 1420-1428. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08. 048
  13. Cheng, L., Gosseries, O., Ying, L., Hu, X., Yu, D., Gao, H., et al. (2013). Assessment of localisation to audi- tory stimulation in post-comatose states: use the patient's own name. BMC Neurol. 13:27. doi:10.1186/ 1471-2377-13-27
  14. Coleman, M. R., Davis, M. H., Rodd, J. M., Robson, T., Ali, A., Owen, A. M., et al. (2009). Towards the routine use of brain imaging to aid the clin- ical diagnosis of disorders of con- sciousness. Brain 132, 2541-2552. doi:10.1093/brain/awp183
  15. Crone, J. S., Ladurner, G., Holler, Y., Golaszewski, S., Trinka, E., and Kro- nbichler, M. (2011). Deactivation of the default mode network as a marker of impaired consciousness: an fMRI study. PLoS ONE 6:e26373. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0026373
  16. Damasio, A., and Meyer, K. (2009). "Consciousness: an overview of the phenomenon and of its possible neural basis," in The Neurology of Consciousness: Cognitive Neuro- science and Neuropathology, eds S. Laureys, and G. Tononi (Oxford: Academic Press), 3-14.
  17. D'Argembeau, A., Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Laureys, S., Del Fiore, G., Degueldre, C., et al. (2005). Self-referential reflective activity and its relationship with rest: a PET study. Neuroimage 25, 616-624. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage. 2004.11.048
  18. Dehaene, S., and Changeux, J. P. (2005). Ongoing spontaneous activity con- trols access to consciousness: a neu- ronal model for inattentional blind- ness. PLoS Biol. 3:e141. doi:10.1371/ journal.pbio.0030141
  19. Demertzi, A., Liew, C., Ledoux, D., Bruno, M.-A., Sharpe, M., Laureys, S., et al. (2009). Dualism persists in the science of mind. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1157, 1-9. doi:10.1111/j.1749- 6632.2008.04117.x
  20. Demertzi, A., Soddu, A., Faymonville, M.-E., Bahri, M.-A., Gosseries, O., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., et al. (2011). Hypnotic modulation of resting state fMRI default mode and extrin- sic network connectivity. Prog. Brain Res. 193, 309-322. doi:10.1016/ B978-0-444-53839-0.00020-X Demertzi, A., Soddu, A., and Lau- reys, S. (2013). Consciousness sup- porting networks. Curr. Opin. Neu- robiol. 23, 239-244. doi:10.1016/j. conb.2012.12.003
  21. Di, H., Boly, M., Weng, X., Ledoux, D., and Laureys, S. (2008). Neuroimag- ing activation studies in the vege- tative state: predictors of recovery? Clin. Med. 8, 502-507. doi:10.7861/ clinmedicine.8-5-502
  22. Di, H. B., Yu, S. M., Weng, X. C., Laureys, S., Yu, D., Li, J. Q., et al. (2007). Cerebral response to patient's own name in the vegeta- tive and minimally conscious states. Neurology 68, 895-899. doi:10.1212/ 01.wnl.0000258544.79024.d0
  23. Feinberg, T. E. (2000). The nested hier- archy of consciousness: a neurobi- ological solution to the problem of mental unity. Neurocase 6, 75-81. doi:10.1080/13554790008402762
  24. Fingelkurts, A. A., Bagnato, S., Boccagni, C., and Galardi, G. D. M. N. (2012). Operational synchrony relates to self-consciousness: evidence from patients in vegetative and mini- mally conscious states. Open Neu- roimag. J. 6, 55-68. doi:10.2174/ 1874440001206010055
  25. Fingelkurts, A. A., Fingelkurts, A. A., and Neves, C. F. H. (2013). Consciousness as a phenomenon in the operational architectonics of brain organization: criticality and self-organization considera- tions. Chaos, Solitons & Fractals 55, 13-31 doi:10.1016/j.chaos.2013. 02.007.
  26. Fins, J. J., and Schiff, N. D. (2010). In the blink of the mind's eye. Hastings Cent. Rep. 40, 21-23. doi:10.1353/ hcr.0.0257
  27. Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., and Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.
  28. U.S.A. 102, 9673-9678. doi:10.1073/ pnas.0504136102
  29. Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., and Raichle, M. E. (2007). Intrinsic fluctuations within cortical systems account for intertrial vari- ability in human behavior. Neuron 56, 171-184. doi:10.1016/j.neuron. 2007.08.023
  30. Fox, M. D., Zhang, D., Snyder, A. Z., and Raichle, M. E. (2009). The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks. J. Neu- rophysiol. 101, 3270-3283. doi:10. 1152/jn.90777.2008
  31. Fransson, P. (2005). Spontaneous low- frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: an fMRI investigation of the resting- state default mode of brain function hypothesis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 26, 15-29. doi:10.1002/hbm.20113
  32. Freeman, W. J. (2007). Indirect biological measures of con- sciousness from field studies of brains as dynamical systems. Neural Netw. 20, 1021-1031. doi:10.1016/j.neunet.2007.09.004
  33. Gantner, I. S., Bodart, O., Laureys, S., and Demertzi, A. (2012). Our rapidly changing understanding of acute and chronic disorders of con- sciousness: challenges for neurolo- gists. Future Neurol. 8, 43-54. doi:10. 2217/fnl.12.77
  34. Giacino, J. T., Ashwal, S., Childs, N., Cranford, R., Jennett, B., Katz, D. I., et al. (2002). The minimally con- scious state: definition and diagnos- tic criteria. Neurology 58, 349-353. doi:10.1212/WNL.58.3.349
  35. Giacino, J. T., Kalmar, K., and Whyte, J. (2004). The JFK coma recovery scale-revised: measurement charac- teristics and diagnostic utility. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 85, 2020-2029. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2004.02.033
  36. Golland, Y., Bentin, S., Gelbard, H., Ben- jamini, Y., Heller, R., Nir, Y., et al. (2007). Extrinsic and intrinsic sys- tems in the posterior cortex of the human brain revealed during nat- ural sensory stimulation. Cereb. Cor- tex 17, 766-777. doi:10.1093/cercor/ bhk030
  37. Gusnard, D. A., and Raichle, M. E. (2001). Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 685-694. doi:10.1038/35094500
  38. Heine, L., Soddu, A., Gomez, F.,Vanhau- denhuyse, A., Tshibanda, L., Thon- nard, M., et al. (2012). Resting state networks and consciousness. Alterations of multiple resting state network connectivity in physiologi- cal, pharmacological and pathologi- cal consciousness states. Front. Psy- chol. 3:295. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012. 00295
  39. Jennett, B., and Plum, F. (1972). Per- sistent vegetative state after brain damage. A syndrome in search of a name. Lancet 1, 734-737. doi:10. 1016/S0140-6736(72)90242-5
  40. John, E. R. (2002). The neurophysics of consciousness. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 39, 1-28. doi:10.1016/S0165- 0173(02)00142-X
  41. Jox, R. J., Bernat, J. L., Laureys, S., and Racine, E. (2012). Disorders of con- sciousness: responding to requests for novel diagnostic and thera- peutic interventions. Lancet Neu- rol. 11, 732-738. doi:10.1016/S1474- 4422(12)70154-0
  42. Laureys, S. (2005). The neural correlate of (un)awareness: lessons from the vegetative state. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 556-559. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.10. 010
  43. Laureys, S., and Boly, M. (2008). The changing spectrum of coma. Nat. Clin. Pract. Neurol. 4, 544-546. doi: 10.1038/ncpneuro0887
  44. Laureys, S., Celesia, G., Cohadon, F., Lavrijsen, J., Leon-Carrrion, J., San- nita, W. G., et al. (2010). Unrespon- sive wakefulness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apal- lic syndrome. BMC Med. 8:68. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-8-68
  45. Laureys, S., Faymonville, M.-E., Deguel- dre, C., Fiore, G. D., Damas, P., Lam- bermont, B., et al. (2000). Auditory processing in the vegetative state. Brain 123, 1589-1601. doi:10.1093/ brain/123.8.1589
  46. Laureys, S., Perrin, F., and Bredart, S. (2007). Self-consciousness in non- communicative patients. Conscious. Cogn. 16, 722-741. doi:10.1016/j. concog.2007.04.004
  47. Laureys, S., Perrin, F., Faymonville, M.- E., Schnakers, C., Boly, M., Bartsch, V., et al. (2004). Cerebral processing in the minimally conscious state. Neurology 63, 916-918. doi:10.1212/ 01.WNL.0000137421.30792.9B
  48. Leech, R., Braga, R., and Sharp, D. J. (2012). Echoes of the brain within the posterior cingulate cortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 215-222. doi:10.1523/ JNEUROSCI.3689-11.2012
  49. Leech, R., and Sharp, D. J. (2013). The role of the posterior cingulate cor- tex in cognition. Brain doi:10.1093/ brain/awt162 (in press).
  50. Majerus, S., Gill-Thwaites, H., Andrews, K., and Laureys, S. (2005). Behav- ioral evaluation of consciousness in severe brain damage. Prog. Brain Res. 150, 397-413. doi:10.1016/S0079- 6123(05)50028-1
  51. Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., and Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wan- dering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315, 393-395. doi:10.1126/ science.1131295
  52. Monti, M. M., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Coleman, M. R., Boly, M., Pickard, J. D., Tshibanda, L., et al. (2010). Will- ful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. N. Engl. J. Med. 362, 579-589. doi:10.1056/ NEJMoa0905370
  53. Norton, L., Hutchison, R. M., Young, G. B., Lee, D. H., Sharpe, M. D., and Mirsattari, S. M. (2012). Dis- ruptions of functional connectivity in the default mode network of comatose patients. Neurology 78, 175-181. doi:10.1212/WNL. 0b013e31823fcd61
  54. Owen, A. M. (2013). Detecting con- sciousness: a unique role for neu- roimaging. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 109-133. doi:10.1146/annurev- psych-113011-143729
  55. Owen, A. M., Coleman, M. R., Boly, M., Davis, M. H., Laureys, S., and Pickard, J. D. (2006). Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Sci- ence 313, 1402. doi:10.1126/science. 1130197
  56. Qin, P., Di, H., Liu, Y., Yu, S., Gong, Q., Duncan, N., et al. (2010). Anterior cingulate activity and the self in dis- orders of consciousness. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 1993-2002. doi:10.1002/ hbm.20989
  57. Sapir, A., d'Avossa, G., McAvoy, M., Shulman, G. L., and Cor- betta, M. (2005). Brain signals for spatial attention predict per- formance in a motion discrim- ination task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 17810-17815. doi:10.1073/pnas.0504678102
  58. Schilbach, L., Eickhoff, S. B., Rotarska- Jagiela, A., Fink, G. R., and Voge- ley, K. (2008). Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relation- ship to the "default system" of the brain. Conscious. Cogn. 17, 457-467. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.013
  59. Seel, R. T., Sherer, M., Whyte, J., Katz, D. I., Giacino, J. T., Rosen- baum, A. M., et al. (2010). Assess- ment scales for disorders of con- sciousness: evidence-based recom- mendations for clinical practice and research. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 91, 1795-1813. doi:10.1016/j.apmr. 2010.07.218
  60. Seth, A. K., Dienes, Z., Cleeremans, A., Overgaard, M., and Pessoa, L. (2008). Measuring consciousness: relating behavioural and neurophys- iological approaches. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 314-321. doi:10.1016/j.tics. 2008.04.008
  61. Soddu, A., Boly, M., Nir,Y., Noirhomme, Q., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Demertzi, A., et al. (2009). Reaching across the abyss: recent advances in functional magnetic resonance imaging and their potential relevance to disorders of consciousness. Prog. Brain Res. 177, 261-274. doi:10.1016/S0079- 6123(09)17718-X Soddu, A., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Demertzi, A., Bruno, M.-A., Tshibanda, L., Di, H., et al. (2011). Resting state activity in patients with disorders of consciousness. Funct. Neurol. 26, 37-43.
  62. Staffen, W., Kronbichler, M., Aichhorn, M., Mair, A., and Ladurner, G. (2006). Selective brain activity in response to one's own name in the persistent vegetative state. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 77, 1383-1384. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2006.095166
  63. Thibaut, A., Bruno, M.-A., Chatelle, C., Gosseries, O., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Demertzi, A., et al. (2012). Meta- bolic activity in external and inter- nal awareness networks in severely brain-damaged patients. J. Reha- bil. Med. 44, 487-494. doi:10.2340/ 16501977-0940
  64. Tian, L., Jiang, T., Liu, Y., Yu, C., Wang, K., Zhou, Y., et al. (2007). The rela- tionship within and between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems indi- cated by resting state correlational patterns of sensory cortices. Neu- roimage 36, 684-690. doi:10.1016/j. neuroimage.2007.03.044
  65. Tononi, G. (2004). An information integration theory of consciousness. BMC Neurosci. 5:42. doi:10.1186/ 1471-2202-5-42
  66. Tononi, G., and Laureys, S. (2009). "The neurology of consciousness: an overview," in The Neurology of Consciousness: Cognitive Neuro- science and Neuropathology, eds S. Laureys, and G. Tononi (Oxford: Academic Press), 375-412.
  67. Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Demertzi, A., Sch- abus, M., Noirhomme, Q., Bredart, S., Boly, M., et al. (2011). Two dis- tinct neuronal networks mediate the awareness of environment and of self. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 570-578. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21488
  68. Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Noirhomme, Q., Tshibanda, L. J., Bruno, M.-A., Boveroux, P., Schnakers, C., et al. (2010). Default network connec- tivity reflects the level of con- sciousness in non-communicative brain-damaged patients. Brain 133, 161-171. doi:10.1093/brain/awp313
  69. Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Schnakers, C., Bredart, S., and Laureys, S. (2008). Assessment of visual pursuit in post- comatose states: use a mirror. J. Neu- rol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 79, 223. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2007.121624
  70. Weissman, D. H., Roberts, K., Visscher, K., and Woldorff, M. G. (2006). The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 971-978. doi:10.1038/nn1727
  71. Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Moran, J. M., Nieto-Castanon, A., Triantafyllou, C., Saxe, R., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2011). Associations and dissoci- ations between default and self- reference networks in the human brain. Neuroimage 55, 225-232. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.048
  72. Zeman, A. (2001). Consciousness. Brain 124, 1263-1289. doi:10.1093/brain/ 124.7.1263