The Origins and Development of Aesthetics
2024, Philosophical Transactions B of the Royal Society
https://doi.org/10.1098/RSTB.2023.0246Abstract
All people (and some other animals) have aesthetic responses to sensory stimulation, responses of emotional pleasure or displeasure. These emotions vary from one person and culture to another, yet they share a common mechanism. To survive, an adaptive animal (as opposed to a tropic animal) needs to become comfortable with normality and to have slight abnormalities draw attention to themselves. Walking through a jungle you need to notice a tiger from a single stripe: if you must wait to see the whole animal, you are unlikely to survive. In Homo sapiens, the brain's adaptive neurochemistry does this naturally, partly because the brain's neuronal networks are structured to react efficiently to fractal structures, structures that shape much of nature. Previous associations may turn a slight variation from normal into feelings of either pleasure or danger. The details of these responses-what is normal and what variations feel like-they will depend upon an individual's experience, but the mechanism is the same, no matter whether a person is tasting a wine, seeing a face or landscape, or hearing a song.
References (87)
- Maurer, D., & Maurer, C. (1988) The World of the Newborn. New York: Basic Books.
- Maurer, C., & Maurer, D. (2020) Pretty Ugly: Why We Like Some Songs, Faces, Foods, Plays, Pictures, Poems, etc., and Dislike Others. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.
- Berlyne, D. E. (1972). Ends and means of experimental aesthetics. Canadian Journal of Psychology / Revue canadienne de psychologie, 26(4), 303-325. https:// doi.org/10.1037/h0082439 (https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0082439)
- Conway, B. R., & Livingstone, M. S. (2007). Perspectives on science and art. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17(4), 476-482.
- Winkielman, P., Halberstadt, J., Fazendeiro, T., & Catty, S. (2006). Prototypes are attractive because they are easy on the mind. Psychol Sci, 17(9), 799-806.
- Zeki, S. (2001). Artistic Creativity and the Brain. New Series, 293(5527), 51-52.
- Steiner, J. (1987).What the neonate can tell us about umami. In Y. Kawamura & M. R. Kare (Eds.), Unami: A Basic Taste (pp. 97-123). New York: Marcel Dekker.
- Steiner, J. E., Glaser, D., Hawilo, M. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2001). Comparative expression of hedonic impact: affective reactions to taste by human infants and other primates. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review, 25(1), 53-74.
- 9 Schaal, B., Marlier, L., & Soussignan, R. (2000). Human foetuses learn odours from their pregnant mother's diet. Chemical Senses, 25(6), 729-737.
- Mennella, J. A., Jagnow, C. P., & Beauchamp, G. K. (2001). Prenatal and postnatal flavor learning by human infants. Pediatrics, 107(6), E88.
- Bunn (1998). Coffee Brewing Guide. https://web.archive.org/web/ 20171226212121/http://www.nacsonline.com/YourBusiness/GettingStarted/ Documents/CoffeeBrewingGuide_Bunn.pdf. (Retrieved 13 July, 2021.)
- Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 199-226. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208
- Langlois, J., & Roggman, L. (1990). Attractive faces are only average. Psycho- logical Science, 1(2), 115-121.
- Vingilis-Jaremko, L., & Maurer, D. (2013). The influence of averageness on children's judgments of facial attractiveness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(4), 624-639. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.03.014
- Halberstadt, J., & Rhodes, G. (2000). The attractiveness of nonface averages: implications for an evolutionary explanation of the attractiveness of average faces. Psychological Science, 11(4), 285-289.
- Halberstadt, J., & Rhodes, G. (2003). It's not just average faces that are attractive: Computer-manipulated averageness makes birds, fish, and automo- biles attractive. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(1), 149-156
- Damon, F., Méary, D., Quinn, P. C., Lee, K., Simpson, E. A., Paukner, A., . . . Pascalis, O. (2017). Preference for facial averageness: Evidence for a common mechanism in human and macaque infants. Scientific Reports, 7, 46303. doi:10.1038/srep46303
- Langlois, J., Roggman, L., & Rieser-Danner, L. (1990). Infants' differential social responses to attractive and unattractive faces. Developmental Psychology, 26, 153-159.
- von Hofsten, O., von Hofsten, C., Sulutvedt, U., Laeng, B., Brennen, T., & Magnussen, S. (2014). Simulating newborn face perception. Journal of Vision, 14(13), 16. doi:10.1167/14.13.1
- Maurer, D., & Salapatek, P. (1976). Developmental changes in the scanning of faces by young infants. Child Development, 47, 523-527.
- Haith, M. M., Bergman, T., & Moore, M. J. (1977). Eye contact and face Scanning in early infancy. Science, 198(4319), 853-855.
- Lewis, T. L., Maurer, D., & Kay, D. (1978). Newborns' central vision: Whole or hole? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 26(1), 193-203.
- Maurer, D. (1983). The scanning of compound figures by young infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 437-448.
- Mondloch, C. J., Lewis, T. L., Budreau, D. R., Maurer, D., Dannemiller, J. L., Stephens, B. R., & Kleiner-Gathercoal, K. A. (1999). Face Perception During Early Infancy. Psychological Science, 10(5), 419-422.
- Cassia, V. M., Turati, C., & Simion, F. (2004). Can a nonspecific bias toward top-heavy patterns explain newborns' face preference? Psychological Science, 15(6), 379-383. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00688.x
- Cassia, V. M., Valenza, E., Simion, F., & Leo, I. (2008). Congruency as a nonspecific perceptual property contributing to newborns' face preference. Child Development, 79(4), 807-820. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01160.x
- Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., Menon, E., Zulian, L., Faraguna, D., & Csibra, G. (2005). Newborns' preference for face-relevant stimuli: effects of contrast polarity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(47), 17245-17250. doi:10.1073/pnas.0502205102
- Simion, F., Valenza, E., Cassia, V., Turati, C., & Umilta, C. (2002). Newborns' preference for up-down asymmetrical configuations. Developmental Science, 5, 427-434.
- Lewis, T. L., & Maurer, D. (1992). The development of the temporal and nasal visual fields during infancy. Vision Res, 32(5), 903-911.
- Komban, S. J., Alonso, J.-M., & Zaidi, Q. (2011). Darks are processed faster than lights. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(23), 8654-8658. doi:10.1523/JNEU- ROSCI.0504-11.201
- Dannemiller, J. L., & Stephens, B. R. (2001). Asymmetries in contrast polarity processing in young human infants. Journal of Vision, 1, 112-125.
- Sai, F. Z. (2005). The role of the mother's voice in developing mother's face preference: Evidence for intermodal perception at birth. Infant and Child Development, 14(1), 29-50. doi:10.1002/icd.376
- Marlier, L., Schaal, B., & Soussignan, R. (1998). Neonatal responsiveness to the odor of amniotic and lacteal fluids: a test of perinatal chemosensory continuity. Child Development, 69(3), 611-623.
- Maurer, D. (2020). Visual development. In J. Lockman & C. Tamis-Lemonda (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Infant Development (pp. 157-185). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
- de Haan, M., Johnson, M. H., Maurer, D., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Recognition of individual faces and average face prototypes by 1-and 3-month-old infants. Cognitive Development, 16(2), 659-678
- Barrera, M. E., & Maurer, D. (1981). Recognition of mother's photographed face by the three-month-old infant. Child Development, 52(2), 714-716. doi:10.2307/1129196
- Messinger, D., & Fogel, A. (2007). The interactive development of social smiling. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 35, 327-366.
- Ahn, Y. A., Önal Ertuğrul, I., Chow, S. M., Cohn, J. F., & Messinger, D. S. (2023). Automated measurement of infant and mother Duchenne facial expres- sions in the Face-to-Face/Still-Face. Infancy, 28(5), 910-929. https://doi.org/ 10.1111/infa.12556
- Watson, J. S., Hayes, L. A., Vietze, P., & Becker, J. (1979). Discriminative infant smiling to orientations of talking faces of mother and stranger. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 28, 92-99.
- Geldart, S., Maurer, D., & Henderson, H. (1999). Effects of the height of the internal features of faces on adults' aesthetic ratings and 5-month-olds' looking times. Perception, 28, i839-850. doi:10.1068/p2943
- Geldart, S. (2008). Tall and good-looking? Journal of Individual Differences, 29(3), 148-156. doi:10.1027/1614-0001.29.3.148
- Cooper, P. A., Geldart, S. S., Mondloch, C. J., & Maurer, D. (2006). Develop- mental changes in perceptions of attractiveness: a role of experience? Develop- mental Science, 9(5), 530-543. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00520.x
- Cooper, P. A., & Maurer, D. (2008). The influence of recent experience on perceptions of attractiveness. Perception, 37, 1216-1226. doi:10.1068/p5865
- Vingilis-Jaremko, L., Ravelo, M., & Maurer, D. (2013). Above average: Percep- tions of attractiveness in children and adults. Journal of Vision, 13, 855
- Glauert, R., Rhodes, G., Byrne, S., Fink, B., & Grammer, K. (2009). Body dissatisfaction and the effects of perceptual exposure on body norms and ideals. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 42(5), 443-452. doi:10.1002/eat.20640
- Devine, S., Germain, N., Ehrlich, S., & Eppinger, B. (2022). Changes in the prevalance of thin bodies bias young women's judgments about body size. Psychological Science. 33(8), 1212-1225.
- Feghelm, D., & Kersting, M. (2005). Rubens and his women (P. Aston, Trans.). Munich: Prestel.
- Hooper, L. H. (1879). The Paris Salon of 1879.I. The Art Journal (1875-1887), 5, 217-219.
- Werner, A. (1955). The great Boug-Bear of academic painting, The Antioch Review, 15(4), 483-495.
- Müller, H., (2009). Fractal scaling models of resonant oscillations in chain systems of harmonic oscillators. Progress in Physics 2, 72
- "Acoustics" in Randel, D. M. (2003). The Harvard Dictionary of Music: Fourth Edition. Harvard University Press.
- Lau, B. K., Lalonde, K., Oster, M. M., & Werner, L. A. (2017). Infant pitch perception: Missing fundamental melody discrimination. Journal of the Acousti- cal Society of America, 141(1), 65. doi:10.1121/1.4973412
- Virtala, P., Huotilainen, M., Partanen, E., Fellman, V., & Tervaniemi, M. (2013). Newborn infants' auditory system is sensitive to Western music chord categories. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 492.
- Masataka, N. (2006). Preference for consonance over dissonance by hearing newborns of deaf parents and of hearing parents. Developmental Science, 9(1), 46-50.
- Spehar, B., Wong, S., van de Klundert, S., Lui, J., Clifford, C. W. G., & Taylor, R. P. (2015). Beauty and the beholder: the role of visual sensitivity in visual preference. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 514. doi:10.3389/ fnhum.2015.00514
- Spehar, B., Clifford, C. W. G., Newell, B. R., & Taylor, R. P. (2003). Universal aesthetic of fractals. Computers & Graphics, 27(5), 813-820. doi:10.1016/ s0097-8493(03)00154-7
- Leleu, A., Rekow, D., Poncet, F., Schaal, B., Durand, K., Rossion, B., & Bau- douin, J. Y. (2019). Maternal odor shapes rapid face categorization in the infant brain. Developmental Science, e12877. doi:10.1111/desc.12877
- Spector, F., & Maurer, D. (2009). Synesthesia: a new approach to understand- ing the development of perception. Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 175-189. doi:10.1037/a0014171
- Simner, J., Mulvenna, C., Sagiv, N., Tsakanikos, E., Witherby, S. A., Fraser, C., . . . Ward, J. (2006). Synaesthesia: The prevalence of atypical cross-modal experiences. Perception, 35(8), 1024-1033. doi:10.1068/p5469
- Sagiv, N., Simner, J., Collins, J., Butterworth, B., & Ward, J. (2006). What is the relationship between synaesthesia and visuo-spatial number forms? Cogni- tion, 101(1), 114-128. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2005.09.004
- Zellner, D. A., & Kautz, M. A. (1990). Color affects perceived odor intensity. Journal of Experimental. Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(2), 391-397.
- Gallagher, M., & Ferrè, E. (2018). The aesthetics of verticality: A gravitational contribution to aesthetic preference. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychol- ogy, 71(12), 2655-2664.
- Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands "feel" touch that eyes see. Nature, 391(6669), 756.
- Kitagawa, N. (2013). Link between Hearing and Bodily Sensations. NTT Technical Review, 11(12).
- Ward, J., & Banissy, M. J. (2015). Explaining mirror-touch synesthesia. Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(2-3), 118-133. doi:10.1080/17588928.2015.1042444
- Ward, J., Buckstep, B., & Tsakanikos, E. (2006). Sound-colour synaesthesia: To what extent does it use cross-modal mechanisms common to us all? Cortex, 42, 264-280.
- Gonzalez-Sanchez, V. E., Zelechowska, A., & Jensenius, A. R. (2018). Corre- spondences between music and involuntary human micromotion during stand- still. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1382.
- Austin, A. M., & Peery, J. C. (1983). Analysis of adult-neonate synchrony during speech and nonspeech. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 57(2), 455-459.
- Condon, W. S., & Sander, L. W. (1974a). Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: interactional participation and language acquisition. Science, 183(4120), 99-101.
- Condon, W., & Sander, L. W. (1974b). Synchrony demonstrated between movements of the neonate and adult speech. Child Development, 45(2), 456-462. 63.
- Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2005). Feeling the beat: movement influ- ences infant rhythm perception. Science, 308(5727), 1430. https://doi.org/10.1126/ science.1110922
- Phillips-Silver, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2008). Vestibular influence on auditory metrical interpretation. Brain and Cognition, 67, 94-102
- Trainor, L. J., Gao, X., Lei, J.-j., Lehtovaara, K., & Harris, L. R. (2009). The primal role of the vestibular system in determining musical rhythm. cortex, 45(1), 35-43.
- Woolhouse, M. H., Tidhar, D., & Cross, I. (2016). Effects on Inter-Personal Memory of Dancing in Time with Others. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 167. https:// doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00167
- Hao, Q., Ora, H., Ogawa, K.-i., Amano, S.-i., & Miyake, Y. (2020). Effects of human synchronous hand movements in eliciting a sense of agency and owner- ship. Scientific reports, 10(1), 1-11.
- Mozart, W. A. (1866). The Letters of Wolfgang Amadeus Mozart (1769-1791) (G. Wallace, Trans.). New York: Hurd & Houghton, p. 63.
- Tarr, B., Launay, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2016). Silent disco: dancing in syn- chrony leads to elevated pain thresholds and social closeness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(5), 343-349.
- McKee, E. (2005). Mozart in the Ballroom: Minuet-Trio Contrast and the Aristocracy in Self-Portrait. Music Analysis, 24(3), 383-434.
- McNeill, W. H. (1997). Keeping together in time : dance and drill in human history (reprint, illustrated ed.). Cambridge, Mass.; London: Harvard University Press.
- Gallup, George (1987). The Gallup Poll: Public Opinion, 1986. p. 104.
- Bowman, L. E., & Lambin, M. W. (1925). Evidences of social relations as seen in types of New York City dance halls. The Journal of Social Forces, 3(2), 286-291).
- British Movietone News (1925). Take Your Partners for the Six-Eight, No- vember 4.
- For a broader context see Maurer, C., & Maurer, D. (2020) Pretty Ugly: Why We Like Some Songs, Faces, Foods, Plays, Pictures, Poems, etc., and Dislike Others. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars. For a more detailed review see Savage PE, Loui P, Tarr B, Schachner A, Glowacki L, Mithen S, Fitch WT (2021). Music as a coevolved system for social bonding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 44, e59: 1-22.
- Thompson, S. & Moussaieff Masson, J. (2010). The Intimate Ape: Orangutans and the Secret Life of a Vanishing Species. New York: Citadel Press.
- Temerlin, M.K. (1975). Lucy: Growing Up Human. Palo Alto, California: Science and Behavior Books.
- Fouts, R, & Mills, S.T. (1997). Next of Kin: What Chimpanzees Have Taught Me about Who We Are. New York: William Morrow.
- Wyrwicka, W. (1993). Social effects on development of food preferences. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 53, 485-485.