Academia.eduAcademia.edu

Outline

Representations reclaimed

2010, Pragmatics and Cognition

https://doi.org/10.1075/P&C.18.2.03PAR

Abstract

Understanding the relationship between concepts and experience seems necessary to specifying the content of experience, yet current theories of concepts do not seem up to the job. With Peter Gärdenfors’s conceptual spaces theory as a foundation and with enactivist philosophy as inspiration, we present a proposed extension to conceptual spaces theory and use it to outline a model of the emergence of concepts and experience. We conclude that neither is ultimately primary but each gives rise to the other: i.e., that they co-emerge. Such a model can then serve as the anchor to a theory of concepts more generally. Concepts are most naturally understood in symbolic and representational terms, while much of experience, in contrast, is non-symbolic and non-representational; yet the conflict between the two will, herein, be shown to be more apparent than real. The main contribution of this paper is to argue for, by means of this account of co-emergence, a continuum between “low-level” mental...

References (51)

  1. Anderson, J. and Lebiere, C. 1998. The Atomic Components of Thought. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  2. Barsalou, L.W., Breazeal, C., and Smith, L.B. 2007. "Cognition as coordinated non-cognition". Cognitive Processing 8(2): 79-91.
  3. Brooks, R.A. 1991a. "Intelligence without reason". In Proceedings, IJCAI-91. San Francisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann, 1-21.
  4. Brooks, R.A. 1991b. "Intelligence without representation". Artificial Intelligence 47: 139-159.
  5. Chalmers, D.J. 1990. "Why Fodor and Pylyshyn were wrong: The simplest refutation". In Pro- gram of the Twelfth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ: Law- rence Erlbaum Associates, 340-347.
  6. Chella, A., Coradeschi, S., Frixione, M., and Saffioti, A. 2004. "Perceptual anchoring via concep- tual spaces". Prooceedings of the AAAI-04 Workshop on Anchoring Symbols to Sensor Data, Menlo Park, CA: AAAI Press, 40-45.
  7. Chella, A., Frixione, M., and Gaglio, S. 2000. "Understanding dynamic scenes". Artificial Intel- ligence 123: 89-132.
  8. Chella, A., Frixione, M., and Gaglio, S. 2008. "A cognitive architecture for robot self-conscious- ness". Artificial Intelligence in Medicine 44(2): 147-154.
  9. Chrisley, R. and Parthemore, J. 2007. "Synthetic phenomenology: Exploiting embodiment to specify the non-conceptual content of visual experience". Journal of Consciousness Studies 14(7): 44-58.
  10. Damasio, A. 2000. The Feeling of What Happens: Body, Emotion and the Making of Consciousness. London: Vintage.
  11. Dennett, D.C. 1991. "Real patterns". Journal of Philosophy 88(1): 27-51.
  12. Dickie, I. 2006. Knowing-which without knowing-that. Unpublished paper presented to the Uni- versity of Sussex Philosophy Society, Brighton, UK.
  13. Evans, G. 1982. Varieties of Reference. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  14. Fodor, J.A. 1975. The Language of Thought. New York, NY: Thomas Y. Crowell.
  15. Fodor, J.A. 2008. LOT 2: The Language of Thought Revisited. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  16. Fodor, J.A. and Pylyshyn, Z.W. 1988. "Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis". In Pinker, S. and Mehler, J. (eds|), Connections and Symbols. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, 3-72.
  17. Gallese, V. and Lakoff, G. 2005. "The brain's concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge". Cognitive Neuropsychology 22(3-4): 455-479.
  18. Geuder, W. and Weisgerber, M. 2002. "Verbs in conceptual space". In G. Katz, S. Reinhard, and P. Reuter (eds), Sinn und Bedeutung 6, Proceedings of the Sixth Meeting of the Gesellschaft für Semantik, Osnabrück, October 2001. Osnabrück: Universität Osnabrück, 69-84.
  19. Gärdenfors, P. 2004. Conceptual Spaces: The Geometry of Thought. London: Bradford Books.
  20. Gärdenfors, P. and Williams, M.-A. 2001. "Reasoning about categories in conceptual spaces". In Proceedings of the Fourteenth International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence. San Francisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann, 385-392.
  21. Grush, R. and Churchland, P. 1995. "Gaps in penrose's toilings". In T. Metzinger (ed), Conscious Experience. Exeter: Imprint Academic, 185-214.
  22. Harnad, S. 1987. Category Perception: The Groundwork of Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press [Chapter "Category Induction and Representation", 535-565].
  23. Harnad, S. 1990. "The symbol grounding problem". Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 42(3): 335-346.
  24. Harnad, S. 2007. "From knowing how to knowing that: Acquiring categories by word of mouth". Presented at Kaziemierz Naturalized Epistemology Workshop (KNEW), Kaziemierz, Po- land, 2 September 2007.
  25. Harnad, S. 2009. "Concepts: The very idea". Presented at the Canadian Philosophical Associa- tion Symposium on Machery's Doing without Concepts. Carleton University, Canada, 27 May 2009.
  26. Harvey, I. 1992. "Untimed and misrepresented: Connectionism and the computer metaphor". Cognitive Science Research Paper 245, Brighton, UK: University of Sussex.
  27. Heidegger, M. 1962. Being and Time. J. Macquarrie and E. Robinson (trans). Oxford: Blackwell.
  28. Held, R. and Hein, A. 1963. "Movement-produced stimulation in the development of visually guided behaviour". Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 56(5): 872-876.
  29. Hemeren, P. 2008. Mind in Action: Action Representation and the Perception of Biological Motion. PhD thesis, University of Lund.
  30. Kirsh, D. 1991. "Today the earwig, tomorrow man?". Artificial Intelligence 47: 161-184.
  31. Langham, M. 1999. An investigation of the role of vehicle conspicuity in the 'looked but failed to see' error in driving. DPhil thesis, University of Sussex.
  32. Machery, E. 2009. Doing Without Concepts. New York: Oxford University Press.
  33. Maturana, H.R. and Varela, F.J. 1992. The Tree of Knowledge. Boston, MA: Shambhala.
  34. Morse, A. and Ziemke, T. 2007. "Cognitive robotics, enactive perception, and learning in the real world". In CogSci 2007 -The 29th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. New York: Erlbaum, 485-490.
  35. Morse, A. and Ziemke, T. Forthcoming. "The somatic sensory hypothesis".
  36. Newell, A. 1980. "Physical symbol systems". Cognitive Science 4(2): 135-183.
  37. Newell, A. and Simon, H.A. 1979. Human Problem Solving. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice- Hall.
  38. Noë, A. 2004. Action in Perception. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  39. Noë, A. 2007. "Magic realism and the limits of intelligibility: What makes us conscious?". Philo- sophical Perspectives 21: 457-474.
  40. Parthemore, J. 1990. Charley: A creative, collaborative short-story writing environment. MSc the- sis, University of Sussex.
  41. Perry, J. 1986. "Thought without representation". Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society 60: 137- 151.
  42. Prinz, J. 2004. Furnishing the Mind: Concepts and Their Perceptual Basis. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  43. Rupert, R.D. 2009. Cognitive Systems and the Extended Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  44. Shusterman, R. 2008. Body Consciousness: A Philosophy of Mindfulness and Somaesthetics. Cam- bridge: Cambridge University Press.
  45. Simons, D.J. and Chabris, C.F. 1999. "Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events". Perception 28: 1059-1074.
  46. Thompson, E. 2007. Mind in Life: Biology, Phenomenology, and the Sciences of Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  47. Thompson, E. and Stapleton, M. 2009. "Making sense of sense-making: Reflections on enactive and extended mind theories". Topoi 28(1): 23-30.
  48. Torey, Z. 2009. The Crucible of Consciousness: An Integrated Theory of Mind and Brain. Cam- bridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  49. Varela, F.J., Thompson, E., and Rosch, E. 1991. The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Hu- man Experience. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  50. Winograd, T. and Flores, C. 1986. Understanding Computers and Cognition: A New Foundation for Design. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
  51. Wittgenstein, L. 2001. Philosophical Investigations. G.E.M. Anscombe (trans). Oxford: Blackwell.